当前位置: 首页 > 医学版 > 期刊论文 > 医药卫生总论 > 美国呼吸和危急护理医学 > 2005年 > 第6期 > 正文
编号:11259644
Habituation of Arousal Responses after Intermittent Hypercapnic Hypoxia in Piglets
     Departments of Medicine and Pediatrics and Child Health, University of Sydney, Sydney Australia

    ABSTRACT

    Rationale: Clinical studies have demonstrated arousal deficits in infants suffering obstructive sleep apnea, and some infant deaths have been attributed to such an arousal deficit. Objectives: To evaluate whether arousal deficits can be induced by intermittent asphyxia during normal development. Methods and Measurements: Young piglets were exposed to intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia for 4 days from age 9.55 ± 0.5 days. Arousal responses were compared between control animals and animals exposed to intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia. Outcome measures included time to arouse after onset of the respiratory stimulus and frequency of arousals during recovery. Main Results: Arousal deficits emerged after successive exposures to hypercapnic hypoxia on Day 1, and were exacerbated on Day 4, although after overnight recovery, the deficit only became evident during the second and subsequent episode of hypercapnic hypoxia. On Day 1, time to arouse increased from 16.9 ± 7.1 seconds in the first epoch to 41.7 ± 28.6 seconds in the fourth epoch (p = 0.004 between cycles, one-way analysis of variance). In the recovery periods after hypercapnic hypoxia, there were 64% fewer arousals than baseline on Day 1 and 90% fewer arousals on Day 4. Respiratory effort, measured by VT across 10 breaths before the arousal, increased from 25.7 ± 7.6 on Day 1 to 29.1 ± 6.8 ml/kg on Day 4 (p < 0.001, two-way analysis of variance, Day 4 vs. Day 1, respectively). Conclusions: These studies demonstrate that acute and chronic arousal deficits can be induced by intermittent asphyxia, on a background of otherwise normal postnatal development.

    Key Words: intermittent hypoxia postnatal OSA SIDS

    Clinical conditions such as obstructive sleep apnea (OSA) may be associated with significant sequelae in young infants. OSA is defined as complete cessation of airflow at the mouth and nose during sleep, despite ongoing diaphragmatic efforts, and is characterized by rapid and repeated onset of perturbations in O2 and CO2 levels as well as sleep disruption (1). The most common signs of OSA in infants are snoring, labored breathing, and profuse sweating (2). Although not well defined for infants, sequelae documented in older children include subtle behavioral changes and secondary phenomena, such as metabolic alkalosis, growth and developmental failure, chronic respiratory failure, and cardiovascular complications (3eC7).

    In infants, OSA is correlated with depression of the arousal response to ventilatory compromise, but treatment to alleviate OSA can reverse this depression (8eC10). Recent studies also suggest that OSA may be linked with the sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS) (11eC13). The most compelling hypothesis regarding SIDS is a failure in the neuroregulation of cardiorespiratory control because of a brainstem abnormality. Neuropathologic studies and more recent genetic studies support the concept that SIDS victims possess underlying vulnerabilities that put them at risk for sudden death (14eC20). It is hypothesized that victims of SIDS, who are believed to die quietly in their sleep, suffer as much from a defect in arousal as from a defect in ventilatory control. The vulnerability of these infants is also believed to be latent until other factors occur, such as a critical period of development and/or an environmental stressor (13, 14, 16, 21, 22). Such a deficit in arousal responses is considered a necessary abnormality for an infant to succumb to SIDS, and it is hypothesized that a brainstem abnormality is responsible for this dysfunction (14, 15). The underlying cause of the brainstem abnormality is not known and may be congenital, caused by a delay in normal maturation, or it may be induced by environmental factors (22).

    Acute exposure to repetitive hypoxia has been shown to result in habituation that is expressed as a decreased frequency of arousal in response to the same stimulus. Fewell and Konduri (23) showed that repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxemia produces an arousal response decrement in sleeping lambs. The stimulus required to induce arousal increased so that the time to arouse and the decrease in SaO2 before arousal were significantly greater after repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxemia. Another study suggested that the carotid chemoreceptors and/or carotid baroreceptors have an important role in initiating arousal from sleep during rapidly developing hypoxemia (23).

    In dogs, chronic exposure to episodic hypoxia via tracheal occlusion depresses arousal responses to asphyxial gases when rebreathing expired air (24). In cats and rats, it was found that there is an important role for dopamine and sympathetic innervation in the carotid body response to hypoxia, and that chronic hypoxia alters sympathetic control (25). A chronic study in dogs found that sleep fragmentation can result in delayed arousal responses and greater arterial oxygen desaturation after acute airway occlusion, leading to the conclusion that the changed responses in OSA are primarily caused by sleep fragmentation (26).

    To examine effects of acute and "chronic" intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia (IHH) exposure on arousal, unsedated piglets were exposed to 6-minute cycles of a hypercapnic hypoxia (HH) gas mixture (8% O2/7% CO2/balance N2), alternating with 6 minutes of air (21% O2/balance N2), for a total time of 48 minutes, daily for 4 days. Arousal responses were monitored on Day 4 and compared with those on Day 1.

    METHODS

    This study used a modification of previously reported protocols (27, 28). Mixed-breed miniature piglets included 11 IHH-treated and 12 control animals (see Table 1). Electrodes were implanted under general anesthetic at 7.83 ± 0.7 days, when piglets weighed 1.68 ± 0.3 kg. Blood pressure and arterial blood gases were obtained from an arterial catheter, EEG and EMG from Grass gold cup electrodes (Model E6GH; Grass Instrument Division, Warwick, RI), and ECG from Teflon-coated stainless-steel wire (A-M Systems, Inc., Carlsborg, WA). All leads were protected in the pockets of jackets that piglets wore after surgery.

    Studies were undertaken in a transparent temperature-regulated polymethylmethacrylate (Perspex; Lucite Solutions, Southampton, UK) box during the piglets' usual "sleep" time. A respiratory circuit, attached to a face mask that was sealed against the snout, provided fresh gas with the relevant concentrations of O2 or CO2. A gas-tight three-way tap was used to switch between gas mixtures with a mean time to stabilization of 13.2 ± 5.7 seconds, which did not differ among gases or studies. Signals were amplified on a Grass Model 8 polygraph (Grass Instruments, Berkshire, UK) and digitized at 100 Hz (Labdat; RHT-InfoDat, Montreal, PQ, Canada).

    Recordings lasted 53 minutes, including a 5-minute baseline in air. The inspired gases were alternated between HH (8% O2, 7% CO2, balance nitrogen) and air (recovery) at 6-minute intervals for a total of 48 minutes: 4 x 6-minute HH cycles and 4 x 6-minute recovery cycles (Figure 1). Blood samples for arterial blood gases were taken before each gas switch and corrected to body temperature (Radiometer, Model ABL 520; Copenhagen, Denmark). Control piglets were subjected to the same protocol, except that inspired gas from tap-switch changes was always medical air. Daily studies were undertaken for 4 days and physiologic data were recorded on Days 1 and 4. Piglets were killed painlessly with an overdose of pentobarbitone (200 mg/kgeC1) after the final study. Ethical approval for the study was obtained from the Animal Ethics Committee of the University of Sydney.

    Identification of Arousals

    The raw data were reviewed visually using a commercially available digital data acquisition and analysis program (Labdat and Anadat; RHT-InfoDat). The raw signals derived from EEG, EMG, airflow, O2, CO2, and blood pressure were reviewed in 60-second epochs and inspected for arousals. An arousal was scored if a change in frequency and amplitude occurred in two or more signals, including EEG. Signal disturbances associated with blood sampling or within 10 seconds of a gas changeover were excluded. All arousals adhering to these criteria were counted, regardless of lower limit in time, because an EEG disruption reflected disturbances at the cortical level. Figure 2 depicts a sample of the raw data. The arousal latency was determined from the point when inspired levels of O2 and CO2 had plateaued at the new levels. The time to the first arousal was the start time of the arousal from the start time of the cycle in seconds. If the piglet failed to arouse during the cycle, a time of 360 seconds (the total time of the cycle) was assigned as the time to arouse for that cycle.

    Results included arousal latency and the number of spontaneous arousals per minute. For the IHH group, spontaneous arousal frequency was determined at baseline and in the recovery cycles, whereas the entire study period was used for the control group. Methods to analyze sleep state are included in the online supplement.

    Ventilation

    VT and E were derived from the calibrated flow signal from the pneumotachograph using a commercially available digital data analysis system (Abreath; RHT-Infodat). Ventilation is expressed in milliliters corrected for weight (ml/kgeC1) and time (ml/kg/minute).

    Sleep

    The montage used to analyze sleep included EEG, EMG, airflow, and blood pressure, with scoring epochs of 15-second duration. Sleep and wakefulness were scored according to the criteria set forth by Anders and Keener (29) to define the states of REM sleep, non-REM sleep, and wake.

    Electrophysiologic and blood gas data were analyzed for sleep and wake, arousal responses, and ventilatory and arterial blood gas parameters. Data are presented as mean ± SD, unless otherwise stated. A p value of 0.05 or less was considered statistically significant. Repeated-measures t tests and analyses of variance (ANOVAs) were performed with Analyze-it for Microsoft Excel (version 1.62; Analyse-it Software, Ltd., Leeds, UK). Comparison of ventilatory data across epochs, and by day, was undertaken using general linear modeling in SPSS version 12.0.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL). Electrophysiologic data were not available for one IHH-exposed piglet.

    RESULTS

    Arousal Latency

    Arousal latency showed both acute and chronic deficits. The time to arouse increased across successive cycles on each study day (p < 0.001). The time to arouse also increased on Day 4 (after 4 consecutive days of IHH exposure) compared with the same cycle on Day 1. Notably, the increase on Day 4 was not apparent during the first HH exposure. The time to arouse in each cycle (apart from the first) was greater on Day 4 compared with the same cycle on Day 1 (p < 0.001, two-way ANOVA; Figure 3).

    Spontaneous Arousals from Sleep

    There were significantly more spontaneous arousals on Day 4 compared with Day 1 in the control animals (p = 0.03), whose results were taken to represent normal maturational changes. This was also observed during baseline for the IHH-treated group where there were more spontaneous arousals during the baseline period on Day 4 compared with Day 1. In the recovery periods after IHH, there were 64% fewer arousals on Day 1 compared with baseline, and 90% fewer arousals compared with baseline on Day 4 in the recovery periods after HH (p = 0.05 by study day, p < 0.001 by cycle; Figure 4).

    Blood Gases

    The changes in PO2 and PCO2 reflect the inspired levels of O2 and CO2 (Table 2). PO2 fell during the HH exposures, and recovered to baseline levels during the intervening recovery periods, with no differences over successive HH exposures, or between Days 1 and 4. PCO2 was elevated during the HH exposures, with associated falls in pH and bicarbonate. During recovery periods, there was hyperventilation, and PCO2 fell below baseline levels, with only partial compensation of the respiratory acidosis induced during the HH exposures. The PaO2 and PaCO2 did not change across epoch or day in air or in HH. Accordingly, the A-a gradient of HH and control animals was never different during cycles of breathing air, and although the gradient increased to 34.9 ± 1.8 during HH exposures (from eC8.4 ± 1.8 in air, p < 0.001), there were no changes across epochs or days in HH or in air.

    Respiratory acidosis became progressively more severe across cycles on each day, although the acidosis was more marked on Day 4. The worsening of the respiratory acidosis over successive cycles only reached statistical significance on Day 4, suggesting that the incomplete compensation with recurrent exposure to HH was further impaired after chronic exposure to IHH as illustrated by changes in base excess (Figure 5 and Figure E1 in the online supplement). For details of blood gas changes across epochs (HH or recovery), see Figures E2eCE4.

    The A-a gradient was calculated, using 0.21 as the FIO2 (low inspired O2 gave similar trends, but less meaningful results). Although the gradient increased during HH exposures (from 8.4 ± 1.8 to 34.9 ± 1.8), the difference was completely explained on the basis of the change in PaO2, and there were no significant effects according to epoch, day, or epoch by day.

    Ventilation

    Mean values for VT in the 10 breaths before each arousal increased across days, although E did not change. Mean values for VT across 10 breaths before the arousal increased from 25.7 ± 7.6 on Day 1 to 29.1 ± 6.8 ml/kg on Day 4 (p = 0.03 between days), although in Epoch 1, the values were not different. On Day 4, values increased from 24.6 ± 7.8 in Epoch 1, to 29.8 ± 7.7, 32.0 ± 6.1, and 30.1 ± 5.7 ml/kg in Epochs 2, 3, and 4, respectively (p < 0.001 for interaction of epoch by day; Figure 6).

    Sleep

    Sleep was assessed in a total of four recovery cycles. As a group, the IHH-exposed piglets spent an increasing proportion of their recovery time asleep from 81.1 ± 12.7 (mean ± SD), 92.0 ± 6.9, 95.5 ± 4.0, 95.9 ± 3.4, and 96.5 ± 3.1 (p = 0.004 between cycles, one-way ANOVA), although individual variability was significant. The control piglets showed a reduction in the time they spent asleep, over successive cycles, with mean values as follows: 91.8 ± 12.3, 83.7 ± 22.4, 85.8 ± 17.9, 76.9 ± 24.3, and 75.7 ± 24.8 (p = 0.0005 between cycles, with one-way ANOVA).

    The IHH-treated piglets spent an increasing proportion of their total sleep time in non-REM sleep as follows: 76.4 ± 19.1, 93.17 ± 13.7, 88.7 ± 13.4, 83.5 ± 24.5, and 87.90 ± 16.8% (p = 0.01 between cycles, one-way ANOVA). The control animals showed no difference in the proportion of their TST in non-REM as follows: 82.87 ± 31.5, 91.35 ± 12.0, 93.11 ± 9.2, 87.53 ± 21.3, and 92.03 ± 11.1% (not significant, one-way ANOVA).

    DISCUSSION

    The most important finding in this study was that a cumulative arousal deficit was induced by daily repetitions of an intermittent respiratory stimulus during early development. Deficits were generated by both acute and chronic exposure. After 4 days of exposure and after an overnight recovery period, we observed only partial recovery of the arousal deficit. The arousal deficit observed in the acute setting was apparently dormant in the chronic setting, but it was induced and exacerbated by repetition of the acute stimulus. Possible explanations include habituation or plasticity of arousal responses, or postexposure depression of arousability. Additional abnormalities affecting spontaneous arousability support the conclusion that the induced changes reflect true depression of arousability. This phenomenon may reflect plasticity of the central nervous system, which is well recognized during early development.

    Habituation of the Arousal Response

    Many features of habituation were observed in this study. These included the following: increase in response decrement with repetition of the stimulus and recovery of native behavior after removal of the stimulus; habituated behavior does not fully recover to native levels of responsiveness, and the habituated behavior dominates when the stimulus is re-presented (30, 31). We demonstrated that the arousal response decreased when the stimulus (respiratory) was repeated, that there was functional recovery of the arousal response when the stimulus presentations ceased, and the habituation recurred when the stimulus was presented again later.

    The second recording, undertaken on the fourth study day, showed that the arousal responses recovered during the first exposure, but habituation recurred, and cumulative depression of arousal was again illustrated. More severe depression of arousal was observed under conditions of chronic exposure, and this was illustrated by an exacerbation of the depression of spontaneous arousability on Day 4 compared with Day 1.

    Other studies have suggested that arousal responses may be subject to habituation. In infants, normal arousals followed a stereotypic pattern of progression from spinal to cortical responses, which is believed to be endogenously regulated, regardless of the sensory input of the stimulus (32, 33). Repeated, tactile stimulation of human infants leads to suppression of cortical arousals so that eventually only spinal responses were observed during sleep (32). Our study was not designed to evaluate whether subcortical responses persisted. The response was specific to the repeated stimulus, because timely responses to other irregular stimuli (e.g., noise and light) persisted, whereas the response decrement to HH was apparent. Although formal demonstration of cumulative long-term habituation requires eight data points to fit a negative exponential equation and failure of recovery of the native response to baseline, the trend of the response decrement was apparent in our dataset (30, 31).

    Depression of Spontaneous Arousability

    With age, piglets and human infants spend an increasing proportion of time in the awake state during early development (34, 35). The animals in this study demonstrated reduced spontaneous arousability during recovery periods, which was marked in the acute setting and further exacerbated in the chronic setting. The deficient arousal response showed only partial recovery on Day 4, and this apparent "dormancy" of the arousal defect fits the pattern of hypothesized vulnerability to SIDS. Candidate locations for the defect include the following: the serotoninergic neurons of the medullary raphe; the cholinergic system in the respiratory network; and neurons in the rostral ventrolateral medulla involving serotonin, substance P, and catecholamines (36, 37), Serotonin is particularly important because of its role in initiating and maintaining respiratory plasticity after intermittent hypoxia as well as initiating homeostatic response to changes in PaCO2 and intracellular pH. Abnormalities have been found in serotoninergic neurons of the ventral medulla in SIDS victims (18, 38, 39).

    Plasticity of Respiratory Sensitivity

    Respiratory plasticity is defined as a persistent change in the neural control system based on prior experience. Plasticity is said to occur when there is a change in the normal course of maturation of the respiratory control network during critical windows of development (38). Experience plays a key role in shaping the development of normal respiratory control in addition to the complex interactions between genes, transcriptional factors, growth factors, and other gene products (40). Plasticity is usually caused by exposure to environmental factors, such as IHH, that cause irreversible alterations in mature respiratory control (40, 41). The piglets in this study were in such a crucial stage of brain development (42). The finding that other responses to IHH were altered in this study suggests that daily exposure to IHH can alter or delay the maturation of other physiologic responses as well. The effect on core body temperature implies effects beyond the respiratory controller, because it is believed that neonates achieve hyperventilation during hypoxia by reducing metabolic rate rather than increasing ventilation (43). Additional studies would be required after more than 24 hours' recovery to determine that true plasticity had occurred.

    The Respiratory Threshold

    A number of studies suggest that arousal may be provoked by respiratory stimuli. The initial arousal was most likely provoked by stimulation of the chemoreceptors. In the acute setting, there was an associated and progressive respiratory acidosis, so chemoreceptor stimulation would have progressively increased, but paradoxically, the arousal latency also increased. These findings suggest an increase in chemoreceptor threshold for arousal.

    The combination of hypercapnia and hypoxemia is a potent stimulus to arouse (44). Parslow and colleagues (45) found that the longer arousal latency during non-REM can be explained by the rate of oxygen desaturation being faster in REM than in non-REM sleep in infants rather than differences in the chemoreceptor threshold. Hypercapnia is also a potent arousal stimulus, which acts through the brainstem reticular activating system (46). It is possible that the chemoreceptor set point was changed. Blanco and coworkers (47) showed that changes in the detection of O2 levels by the peripheral chemoreceptors requires 24 to 31 hours of continuous exposure to new O2 concentrations, but sensory adaptation after presentation of an intermittent respiratory stimulus has not been evaluated.

    Further evidence that the threshold of respiratory stimulation had increased was provided by the increase in VT preceding arousals on Day 4 compared with Day 1. These data fit with the model of Gleeson and colleagues (48), who proposed that the predominant respiratory stimulus for arousal is the amplitude of effort rather than chemostimulation.

    Possible Implications for Infants

    It is hypothesized that victims of SIDS, who presumably die quietly in their sleep, suffer as much or perhaps more from a defect in arousal than in ventilatory control (13, 14, 21, 22). The prone sleeping position is a significant risk factor for SIDS (41). A number of studies have now demonstrated that blunted arousal responses, together with altered sleep architecture and autonomic function, are a feature of the prone sleeping position (13, 49, 50).

    Nonetheless, there is also evidence that HH occurs secondary to rebreathing of expired air in the prone sleeping position (41, 51eC53). Studies on Day 1 could reflect noxious respiratory stimuli experienced by infants sleeping prone for the first time, which is associated with a 14- to 19-fold increased risk for SIDS (54). Studies on Day 4 would reflect a more chronic stimulus, such as may be observed in association with OSA, and which has also been associated with subsequent SIDS (13, 14, 55). Neuropathologic changes in the same infants suggest chronic tissue hypoxia, which supports the latter mechanism.

    Infants with diminished ventilatory responses to hypercapnia or hypoxia fail to arouse to hypercapnic or hypoxic stimuli (56). Such abnormalities of the ventilatory responses have been observed in infants with apparent life-threatening events, and they have been observed in association with OSA in infants (8, 9, 21). Because treatment of OSA apparently reverses this depression (10), the deficit in the arousal response appears to be inducible. Habituation of the arousal response after postnatal exposure to a noxious respiratory stimulus has been illustrated in several studies (23, 57eC59). The hypothesized brainstem abnormality responsible for abnormal arousability may be congenital, but the results of this study suggest that environmental factors can induce arousal deficits in otherwise normal subjects (15, 18).

    It remains possible that genotype ultimately determines whether the response to repeated episodes of HH is favorable or unfavorable, and responses characteristic of polymorphisms relevant to SIDS may only be elicited if repetitive episodes of intermittent HH are experienced during developmentally vulnerable periods. Risk factors such as OSA (which may have another genetic basis) and prone sleep position may only place infants at high risk for fatal consequences during sleep if they interact with specific "vulnerable" genotypes.

    Other Considerations

    The deficit in spontaneous arousals after IHH was associated with an increase in the proportion of non-REM sleep. OSA in human infants induces changes in sleep architecture, and it has been claimed that alleviation of the symptoms of OSA normalizes sleep architecture (60). Not all studies show consistent results regarding state-dependency for arousals or arousal thresholds (23, 58, 59). Obstructive apneas occur more commonly during REM, and studies have found a reduction in the number of spontaneous arousals during REM in infants (8). Also, in dogs, asphyxic arousal is delayed in REM compared with non-REM (58, 61). Harding and colleagues (58) and Johnston and colleagues (59) found that arousal is state-dependent in lambs with a postnatal nadir in arousal responses at age 7 to 12 days. This finding suggests that the arousal mechanism is particularly vulnerable to failure during REM after adverse stimulation at a particular stage of postnatal development.

    The piglets in this study displayed normal early postnatal development, with the only variant to this being their study exposure to HH to simulate ventilatory compromise. Piglets were selected as the animal model for this study because their early postnatal development most closely approximates that of the human infant (35, 42, 62). The age range of 9 to 13 days over the period of study was carefully selected as a developmental stage equivalent to 2 to 6 months in the human infant, an age of peak vulnerability to SIDS (13), and an age of increased vulnerability to respiratory compromise (63).

    The associated changes in arterial gases with a progressive acidosis suggest that the repeated insults were associated with inadequate ventilatory compensation rather than habituation (64). The intermittent nature of the stimulus was apparently important because the adaptive strategies usually seen in neonates experiencing hypoxia, such as reduced metabolic rate and a fall in body temperature, were not sufficient to protect against the insult, and in such circumstances, those strategies may underlie, rather than protect against, hypoxic injury (64).

    Limitations of the Study

    Our definition of arousal may show a lower frequency of arousal compared with other studies because our definition was essentially that of a behavioral arousal and required involvement or more channels than EEG alone. Infants arouse in a sequential manner, beginning with presumptive brainstem responses before cortical arousal, with subcortical arousals sometimes occurring without cortical arousal, and the ability to detect subcortical arousals would be required to show this phenomenon (33, 65).

    Although our study provides a model for clinical diseases such as OSA and environmental factors such as prone sleeping, the results can only be partly extrapolated to clinical settings. The mechanisms stimulating arousal in our study animals would have included alterations in autonomic control independent of mechanoreceptor stimulation and stimulation of the olfactory system, both of which are believed to participate in the generation of arousal (61, 66). Arousal from sleep in response to upper airway obstruction, such as occurs in OSA, almost certainly involves mechanoreceptors as well as chemoresponses (24, 48, 67).

    It remains possible that effector fatigue was a component of the response, but we had no measurement that would allow documentation of respiratory muscle fatigue. However, the increased ventilatory efforts preceding arousal on Day 4 suggest that this is unlikely.

    We excluded effects caused by habituation to the study environment by including a control group that underwent all study procedures except HH gas exposure. Although the study environment permitted auditory and visual stimulation, chronic noise stimulation does not induce any sustained changes in the cardiovascular system and is unlikely to have contributed to our results (68).

    Four days of exposure may be considered a relatively brief duration for chronic exposure, and the finding that this was able to induce an arousal deficit is important. We did not explore whether such an arousal deficit continues to progress with more prolonged exposure or the sequence or time course over which the deficit develops or "recovers."

    More rigorous testing of the habituation hypothesis would require additional studies, such as a change in study context (e.g., removal from the study environment) to show that this led to renewal of the native response to HH exposure, and that reinstatement in the study environment induced rehabituation. Dishabituation may also be demonstrable, if simultaneous delivery of another stimulus with HH would allow reassertion of the native response to HH.

    Conclusions

    These studies have demonstrated that cumulative arousal deficits can be induced after acute (four episodes on 1 day) and chronic (daily for 4 days) exposure to IHH. The arousal deficits were complex, and included changes in the chemostimulation threshold, possible habituation, depression of spontaneous arousability, and dormancy, so that elements of the deficit were not apparent until a subsequent HH exposure occurred. Extrapolating these results to the clinical situation provides data consistent with studies indicating that repeated episodes of OSA induce arousal deficits in infants. Additional information from this study suggests that not all of the deficits would be demonstrable if the infants were not examined during a relevant stress situation that would mimic prior arousal stimuli.

    Acknowledgments

    The authors gratefully acknowledge Prof. Colin Sullivan's critical input to these studies.

    This article has an online supplement, which is accessible from this issue's table of contents at www.atsjournals.org

    REFERENCES

    Kahn A, Blum D, Waterschoot P, Engelman E, Smets P. Effects of obstructive sleep apneas on transcutaneous oxygen pressure in control infants, siblings of sudden infant death syndrome victims, and near miss infants: comparison with the effects of central sleep apneas. Pediatrics 1982;70:852eC857.

    Kahn A, Groswasser J, Sottiaux M, Rebuffat E, Franco P. Mechanisms of obstructive sleep apneas in infants. Biol Neonate 1994;65:235eC239.

    Brouillette RT, Fernbach SK, Hunt CE. Obstructive sleep apnea in infants and children. J Pediatr 1982;100:31eC40.

    Leiberman A, Tal A, Brama I, Sofer S. Obstructive sleep apnea in young infants. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 1988;16:39eC44.

    Singer LP, Saenger P. Complications of pediatric obstructive sleep apnea. Otolaryngol Clin North Am 1990;23:665eC676.

    Freezer NJ, Bucens IK, Robertson CF. Obstructive sleep apnoea presenting as failure to thrive in infancy. J Paediatr Child Health 1995;31:172eC175.

    Gislason T, Benediktsdottir B. Snoring, apneic episodes, and nocturnal hypoxemia among children 6 months to 6 years old. An epidemiologic study of lower limit of prevalence. Chest 1995;107:963eC966.

    McNamara F, Issa FG, Sullivan CE. Arousal pattern following central and obstructive breathing abnormalities in infants and children. J Appl Physiol 1996;81:2651eC2657.

    Harrington C, Kirjavainen T, Teng A, Sullivan CE. Altered autonomic function and reduced arousability in apparent life-threatening event infants with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 2002;165:1048eC1054.

    McNamara F, Sullivan CE. Effects of nasal CPAP therapy on respiratory and spontaneous arousals in infants with OSA. J Appl Physiol 1999;87:889eC896.

    Engelberts AC. The role of obstructive apnea in sudden infant death syndrome and apparent life threatening event. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol 1995;32:S59eCS62.

    Tishler PV, Redline S, Ferrette V, Hans MG, Altose MD. The association of sudden unexpected infant death with obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1996;153:1857eC1863.

    Kahn A, Sawaguchi T, Sawaguchi A, Groswasser J, Franco P, Scaillet S, Kelmanson I, Dan B. Sudden infant deaths: from epidemiology to physiology. Forensic Sci Int 2002;130:S8eC20.

    Sawaguchi T, Franco P, Kato I, Shimizu S, Kadhim H, Groswasser J, Sottiaux M, Togari H, Kobayashi M, Takashima S, et al. From physiology to pathology: arousal deficiency theory in sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS)eCwith reference to apoptosis and neuronal plasticity. Forensic Sci Int 2002;130:S37eCS43.

    Sparks DL, Hunsaker JC III. Neuropathology of sudden infant death (syndrome): literature review and evidence of a probable apoptotic degenerative cause. Childs Nerv Syst 2002;18:568eC592.

    Filiano JJ, Kinney HC. A perspective on neuropathologic findings in victims of the sudden infant death syndrome: the triple-risk model. Biol Neonate 1994;65:194eC197.

    Ozawa Y, Takashima S. Developmental neurotransmitter pathology in the brainstem of sudden infant death syndrome: a review and sleep position. Forensic Sci Int 2002;130:S53eCS59.

    Sawaguchi T, Franco P, Kato I, Shimizu S, Kadhim H, Groswasser J, Sottiaux M, Togari H, Kobayashi M, Takashima S, et al. From epidemiology to physiology and pathology: apnea and arousal deficient theories in sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS)eCwith particular reference to hypoxic brainstem gliosis. Forensic Sci Int 2002;130:S21eCS29.

    Sawaguchi T, Franco P, Kato I, Shimizu S, Kadhim H, Groswasser J, Sottiaux M, Togari H, Kobayashi M, Nishida H, et al. Interaction between apnea, prone sleep position and gliosis in the brainstems of victims of SIDS. Forensic Sci Int 2002;130:S44eCS52.

    Narita N, Narita M, Takashima S, Nakayama M, Nagai T, Okado N. Serotonin transporter gene variation is a risk factor for sudden infant death syndrome in the Japanese population. Pediatrics 2001;107:690eC692.

    McCulloch K, Brouillette RT, Guzzetta AJ, Hunt CE. Arousal responses in near-miss sudden infant death syndrome and in normal infants. J Pediatr 1982;101:911eC917.

    Hunt CE. The cardiorespiratory control hypothesis for sudden infant death syndrome. Clin Perinatol 1992;19:757eC771.

    Fewell JE, Konduri GG. Influence of repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxaemia on the arousal and cardiopulmonary response to rapidly developing hypoxaemia in lambs. J Dev Physiol 1989;11:77eC82.

    Kimoff RJ, Brooks D, Horner RL, Kozar LF, Render-Teixeira CL, Champagne V, Mayer P, Phillipson EA. Ventilatory and arousal responses to hypoxia and hypercapnia in a canine model of obstructive sleep apnea. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1997;156:886eC894.

    Bisgard GE. Carotid body mechanisms in acclimatization to hypoxia. Respir Physiol 2000;121:237eC246.

    Brooks D, Horner RL, Kimoff RJ, Kozar LF, Render-Teixeira CL, Phillipson EA. Effect of obstructive sleep apnea versus sleep fragmentation on responses to airway occlusion. Am J Respir Crit Care Med 1997;155:1609eC1617.

    Waters KA, Tinworth KD. Effect of stimulus cycle time on acute respiratory responses to intermittent hypercapnic hypoxia in unsedated piglets. J Appl Physiol 2003;94:2465eC2474.

    Waters KA, Tinworth KD. Depression of ventilatory responses after daily, cyclic hypercapnic hypoxia in piglets. J Appl Physiol 2001;90:1065eC1073.

    Anders TF, Keener M. Developmental course of nighttime sleep-wake patterns in full-term and premature infants during the first year of life. I. Sleep 1985;8:173eC192.

    McSweeney FK, Murphy ES. Criticisms of the satiety hypothesis as an explanation for within-session decreases in responding. J Exp Anal Behav 2000;74:347eC361.

    McSweeney FK, Swindell S. Common processes may contribute to extinction and habituation. J Gen Psychol 2002;129:364eC400.

    McNamara F, Wulbrand H, Thach BT. Habituation of the infant arousal response. Sleep 1999;22:320eC326.

    Lijowska AS, Reed NW, Chiodini BA, Thach BT. Sequential arousal and airway-defensive behavior of infants in asphyxial sleep environments. J Appl Physiol 1997;83:219eC228.

    Galland BC, Peebles CM, Bolton DP, Taylor BJ. Sleep state organization in the developing piglet during exposure to different thermal stimuli. Sleep 1993;16:610eC619.

    Scott SC, Inman JD, Moss IR. Ontogeny of sleep/wake and cardiorespiratory behavior in unanesthetized piglets. Respir Physiol 1990;80:83eC101.

    Hilaire G, Duron B. Maturation of the mammalian respiratory system. Physiol Rev 1999;79:325eC360.

    Richerson GB, Wang W, Tiwari J, Bradley SR. Chemosensitivity of serotonergic neurons in the rostral ventral medulla. Respir Physiol 2001;129:175eC189.

    Mitchell GS, Johnson SM. Neuroplasticity in respiratory motor control. J Appl Physiol 2003;94:358eC374.

    Wang W, Tiwari JK, Bradley SR, Zaykin RV, Richerson GB. Acidosis-stimulated neurons of the medullary raphe are serotonergic. J Neurophysiol 2001;85:2224eC2235.

    Carroll JL. Developmental plasticity in respiratory control. J Appl Physiol 2003;94:375eC389.

    Harper RM, Kinney HC, Fleming PJ, Thach BT. Sleep influences on homeostatic functions: implications for sudden infant death syndrome. Respir Physiol 2000;119:123eC132.

    Dobbing J. 1981. The later development of the brain and its vulnerability. In J. Davis and J. Dobbing, editors. Scientific Foundations of Paediatrics. University Park Press, Baltimore. 744eC59.

    Mortola JP. How newborn mammals cope with hypoxia. Respir Physiol 1999;116:95eC103.

    Fewell JE, Konduri GG. Repeated exposure to rapidly developing hypoxemia influences the interaction between oxygen and carbon dioxide in initiating arousal from sleep in lambs. Pediatr Res 1988;24:28eC33.

    Parslow PM, Harding R, Cranage SM, Adamson TM, Horne RS. Ventilatory responses preceding hypoxia-induced arousal in infants: effects of sleep-state. Respir Physiolo Neurobiol 2003;136:235eC247.

    Siesjo BK. Cerebral metabolic rate in hypercarbiaeCa controversy. Anesthesiology 1980;52:461eC465.

    Blanco CE, Hanson MA, McCooke HB. Effects on carotid chemoreceptor resetting of pulmonary ventilation in the fetal lamb in utero. J Dev Physiol 1988;10:167eC174.

    Gleeson K, Zwillich CW, White DP. The influence of increasing ventilatory effort on arousal from sleep. Am Rev Respir Dis 1990;142:295eC300.

    Galland BC, Reeves G, Taylor BJ, Bolton DP. Sleep position, autonomic function, and arousal. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 1998;78:F189eCF194.

    Horne RS, Franco P, Adamson TM, Groswasser J, Kahn A. Effects of body position on sleep and arousal characteristics in infants. Early Hum Dev 2002;69:25eC33.

    Constantin E, Waters KA, Morielli A, Brouillette RT. Head turning and face-down positioning in prone-sleeping premature infants. J Pediatr 1999;134:558eC562.

    Kemp JS, Livne M, White DK, Arfken CL. Softness and potential to cause rebreathing: Differences in bedding used by infants at high and low risk for sudden infant death syndrome. J Pediatr 1998;132:234eC239.

    Waters KA, Gonzalez A, Jean C, Morielli A, Brouillette RT. Face-straight-down and face-near-straight-down positions in healthy, prone-sleeping infants. J Pediatr 1996;128:616eC625.

    Cote A, Gerez T, Brouillette RT, Laplante S. Circumstances leading to a change to prone sleeping in sudden infant death syndrome victims. Pediatrics 2000;106:E86.

    Kahn A, Groswasser J, Rebuffat E, Sottiaux M, Blum D, Foerster M, Franco P, Bochner A, Alexander M, Bachy A, et al. Sleep and cardiorespiratory characteristics of infant victims of sudden death: a prospective case-control study. Sleep 1992;15:287eC292.

    Hunt CE, McCulloch K, Brouillette RT. Diminished hypoxic ventilatory responses in near-miss sudden infant death syndrome. J Appl Physiol 1981;50:1313eC1317.

    Ward SL, Bautista DB, Keens TG. Hypoxic arousal responses in normal infants. Pediatrics 1992;89:860eC864.

    Harding R, Jakubowska AE, McCrabb GJ. Arousal and cardiorespiratory responses to airflow obstruction in sleeping lambs: effects of sleep state, age, and repeated obstruction. Sleep 1997;20:693eC701.

    Johnston RV, Grant DA, Wilkinson MH, Walker AM. Repetitive hypoxia rapidly depresses arousal from active sleep in newborn lambs. J Physiol 1998;510:651eC659.

    McNamara F, Sullivan CE. Sleep-disordered breathing and its effects on sleep in infants. Sleep 1996;19:4eC12.

    Berthon-Jones M, Sullivan CE. Ventilation and arousal responses to hypercapnia in normal sleeping humans. J Appl Physiol 1984;57:59eC67.

    Dickerson J, Dobbing J. Prenatal and postnatal growth and development of the central nervous system of the pig. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1967;166:384eC395.

    Cote A, Porras H, Meehan B. Age-dependent vulnerability to carotid chemodenervation in piglets. J Appl Physiol 1996;80:323eC331.

    Waters KA, Gozal D. Responses to hypoxia during early development. Respir Physiolo Neurobiol 2003;136:115eC129.

    McNamara F, Wulbrand H, Thach BT. Characteristics of the infant arousal response. J Appl Physiol 1998;85:2314eC2321.

    White SG, Fletcher EC, Miller CC III. Acute systemic blood pressure elevation in obstructive and nonobstructive breath hold in primates. J Appl Physiol 1995;79:324eC330.

    Berry RB, Gleeson K. Respiratory arousal from sleep: mechanisms and significance. Sleep 1997;20:654eC675.

    Bao G, Metreveli N, Fletcher EC. Acute and chronic blood pressure response to recurrent acoustic arousal in rats. Am J Hypertens 1999;12:504eC510.(Karen A. Waters and Kelli)