当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《内分泌学杂志》 > 2005年第9期 > 正文
编号:11295350
Enduring, Handling-Evoked Enhancement of Hippocampal Memory Function and Glucocorticoid Receptor Expression Involves Activation of the Corti
http://www.100md.com 《内分泌学杂志》
     Departments of Anatomy/Neurobiology (K.A.F., K.L.B., S.A.-E., B.A.S., B.J.K., T.Z.B.) and Pediatrics (T.Z.B.), University of California, Irvine, California 92697-4475

    Hebrew University, Jerusalem and Kaplan Medical Center (S.A.-E.), Rehovoth, Israel

    Abstract

    Early-life experience, including maternal care, influences hippocampus-dependent learning and memory throughout life. Handling of pups during postnatal d 2–9 (P2–9) stimulates maternal care and leads to improved memory function and stress-coping. The underlying molecular mechanisms may involve early (by P9) and enduring reduction of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) expression and subsequent (by P45) increase in hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression. However, whether hypothalamic CRF levels influence changes in hippocampal GR expression (and memory function), via reduced CRF receptor activation and consequent lower plasma glucocorticoid levels, is unclear. In this study we administered selective antagonist for the type 1 CRF receptor, NBI 30775, to nonhandled rats post hoc from P10–17 and examined hippocampus-dependent learning and memory later (on P50–70), using two independent paradigms, compared with naive and vehicle-treated nonhandled, and naive and antagonist-treated handled rats. Hippocampal GR and hypothalamic CRF mRNA levels and stress-induced plasma corticosterone levels were also examined. Transient, partial selective blockade of CRF1 in nonhandled rats improved memory functions on both the Morris watermaze and object recognition tests to levels significantly better than in naive and vehicle-treated controls and were indistinguishable from those in handled (naive, vehicle-treated, and antagonist-treated) rats. GR mRNA expression was increased in hippocampal CA1 and the dentate gyrus of CRF1-antagonist treated nonhandled rats to levels commensurate with those in handled cohorts. Thus, the extent of CRF1 activation, probably involving changes in hypothalamic CRF levels and release, contributes to the changes in hippocampal GR expression and learning and memory functions.

    Introduction

    GENETIC FACTORS INTERACT with early-life experience to shape stress responses and learning and memory functions during adulthood (1, 2, 3, 4). In the immature rodent, maternal input influences cognitive function and neuroendocrine stress responses during adulthood (5, 6, 7, 8, 9). Enhanced maternal care, elicited by brief, daily separation of rat pups from the dam (handling) (6, 7, 10, 11) for the first 1–3 postnatal weeks leads to long-term reduction in stress responses (12) and improved spatial learning and memory (13, 14). The molecular foundations of this long-lasting neuroplasticity may involve enduring reduction of corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) expression in the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus (PVN) and increased glucocorticoid receptor (GR) expression in the hippocampus (11, 15).

    In exploring the mechanisms by which early-life experience regulates hippocampal GR expression and memory function, we have previously shown that handling from postnatal d 2–9 (P2–9) is sufficient to influence CRF and GR expression long term (11). We also found that hypothalamic CRF mRNA is down-regulated by the end of the handling paradigm on P9 (11). This reprogramming of hypothalamic CRF gene expression is an early change in the molecular cascade initiated by handling, because lower stress-evoked ACTH and corticosterone (CORT) levels are not evident until the third postnatal week. The reduced neuroendocrine stress response, in turn, is followed by elevation of hippocampal GR mRNA levels (by P45) and improved hippocampal-mediated cognitive function (7, 14).

    This sequence of changes suggests the possibility that they may be linked. Reduced CRF levels should promote less peptide release in response to stress and thus diminished activation of the major pituitary CRF receptor, CRF1. The resulting reduction in plasma CORT levels should contribute to up-regulation of GR expression (16). An alternative hypothesis suggests that enhanced maternal input influences hippocampal GR expression (and function) independently from changes in CRF expression in the PVN.

    To distinguish between these alternatives, we reasoned that if reduced activation of CRF1 by CRF (and consequent lower stress-evoked plasma CORT levels) immediately after the handling paradigm is required for long-lasting up-regulation of GR expression in the hippocampus and improved learning and memory functions, then partially blocking CRF1 receptors in nonhandled rats should recapitulate these changes. We took advantage of the fact that the enduring increase in GR expression in rats handled from P2–9 commences sometime between P23 and P45 (11), supporting the existence of a window of opportunity to intervene, and thus administered a selective CRF1 antagonist (NBI 30775) to nonhandled rats from P10–17. In this study we show that hippocampal GR expression and memory function and hypothalamic CRF levels of nonhandled rats are modified to levels typical of handled rats after this transient, systemic, post hoc administration of CRF1 antagonist.

    Materials and Methods

    Animals

    Timed-pregnant Sprague Dawley rats were housed in a quiet, uncrowded animal facility under a 12-h light, 12-h dark cycle (lights on at 0700 h), with food and water provided ad libitum. Parturition was checked once a day, and the day of birth was considered P0. On P1, litters were mixed (to minimize effects of genetic or prenatal variability) and adjusted to six males and six females. Early-life rearing conditions, either handled or nonhandled, were assigned to each litter. Twelve litters were used. All experimental procedures were approved by university animal care committee and conformed to National Institutes of Health guidelines (http://oacu.od.nih.gov/NIHpolicy).

    Experimental design

    Sprague Dawley male rats were divided into six experimental groups: handled (H); nonhandled (NH); handled pups that were implanted with an Alzet minipump filled with vehicle (distilled water) on P10 (H+Veh); nonhandled rats that were implanted with vehicle-containing minipump on P10 (NH+Veh); nonhandled rats that were implanted with a pump containing the CRF1-antagonist, NBI 30775 (NH+Antag); and handled rats implanted with NBI 30775-containing pump (H+Antag).

    Handling, a brief separation from the dam for 15 min, was carried out daily at 0830 h from P2–9 according to procedures previously described (11, 15); we have found that this duration of the procedure suffices to alter CRF and GR expression long term (11). During this time, nonhandled rats remained undisturbed with the dam. Cages were not changed for any of the experimental conditions from P2–9. On P10, all litters were returned to routine animal facility care (cage changes twice a week) until weaning on P21. On P21, all groups were housed two or three rats per cage.

    Administration of the CRF1-antagonist

    The selective nonpeptide CRF1 antagonist, NBI 30775 (3-[6-(dimethylamino)-4-methyl-pyrid-3-yl]-2,5-dimethyl-N,N-dipropyl-pyrazolo[2,3-a]-pyrimidin-7-amine, also formerly known as R121919) was a gift from Dr. Dimitri Grigoriadis (Neurocrine Biosciences, San Diego, CA). The compound was dissolved in warm distilled water, and the pH was adjusted. The solution (4 mg/ml) was loaded into a microosmotic Alzet pump (model 1007D, Alzet Corp., Cupertino, CA), calibrated to release 0.5 μl/h (2.5 mg/kg·d) (17) over 7 d. Before surgery, two P10 rats (one about to undergo surgery and one nonimplanted control) were removed from the home cage, placed in a separate cage, and kept euthermic on a warming pad (18, 19). Osmotic Alzet minipumps were implanted sc in the nape of the neck using halothane anesthesia (18). Implantation surgery was performed between 0830–1030 h and was completed within 5 min of disturbance. Pups were allowed to recover in the separate cage containing the control pup before being returned to the home cage. CRF1 antagonist was continuously infused from P10–17. The minipumps were removed on the day of weaning, P21 (19). When pumps were removed, pups were treated in the same manner as during implantation surgery.

    Morris watermaze test

    Hippocampus-dependent learning and memory were tested between P50 and P70 without knowledge of early-life treatment. Spatial memory was tested using a modified Morris watermaze procedure (18, 20). A circular pool (2 m in diameter, 0.6 m deep) was filled with 24 C water made opaque by the addition of powdered milk. A transparent escape platform, 13 cm in diameter, was positioned 2 cm below the surface of the water. Rats were subjected to 2 consecutive days of place training (seven trials per day) to familiarize them with locating and perching on the platform using visual cues in the testing room. During training, the escape platform remained in a fixed location in the center of the northwest quadrant of the pool. At the beginning of each trial, the rat was lowered into the water facing the inside wall of the pool. If the rat did not reach the platform in 60 sec, it was guided to the platform by the experimenter. Rats were kept on the platform for 15 sec and then returned to a holding cage. On the day of testing (d 3), the hidden platform was placed in the southwest quadrant of the pool. Rats were placed in the water at one of three random start positions, which rotated among the remaining quadrants. Rats were subjected to six trials. Latency to reach the escape platform (seconds) was recorded.

    Object recognition test

    The Morris watermaze involves potentially stressful elements of being forced to swim (18, 20) and may thus be confounded by the differential effects of early-life treatment of the groups’ stress responses. Therefore, a second memory test that requires an intact hippocampus and is relatively less stressful than the watermaze was chosen. The 2-d object recognition procedure that tests hippocampus-dependent retrieval function (18, 21) was administered in P50–70 rats. Briefly, adult rats were tested in a 52 x 27 x 21-cm (length x width x height) Plexiglass cage that was lined with opaque paper, leaving the front panel transparent to permit observation by the investigator. The day before sample training, subjects were habituated to the testing cage in a quiet testing room for 20 min. On d 1, each animal was given one sample trial during which it was allowed to explore two objects, randomly placed 6 cm from a wall. Twenty-four hours later, the test trial was administered. During the test trial, the animal was allowed 5 min to explore a duplicate of an object from the sample trial (to avoid olfactory cues) and a novel object. In both sample and test trials, the duration of exploration of each object, defined as sniffing with the animal’s nose in direct contact or within 2 cm of the object, was recorded.

    Behavioral data analysis

    The mean latency in seconds (±SE) to reach the Morris watermaze platform per trial was calculated for each group. Significant differences were determined among groups (all groups were included for each analysis) using one-factor ANOVA with repeated measures (trials as the within-subject variable and early-life treatment as the between-subjects variable) followed by Bonferonni’s post hoc test (unless otherwise noted). For the object recognition test, significant differences among groups in time spent exploring each object were analyzed using one-factor ANOVA with Bonferonni’s post hoc test (unless otherwise noted). In all cases, P < 0.05 was considered significant (PRISM, GraphPad, Inc., San Diego, CA). For descriptive statistics, means and SEs were calculated.

    Animal/tissue preparation and hormonal assay

    Approximately half the rats were killed via decapitation (0800–0900 h), and trunk blood was collected to measure baseline CORT levels. Brains were rapidly removed, immediately frozen on powdered dry ice, and stored at –80 C for mRNA analysis. The remaining animals were subjected to restraint stress. Animals were restrained in an acrylic chamber for 20 min. Thirty minutes after the onset of the restraint stress, trunk blood was collected. Plasma CORT levels were determined using a commercially available RIA kit (ICN Biochemicals, Irvine, CA) as previously described (22). The CORT assay sensitivity was 0.16 μg/dl. Interassay variability, determined by three dilutions of adult rat plasma, averaged 0.5 μg/dl.

    In situ hybridization histochemistry (ISH)

    Expression of hippocampal GR and PVN CRF mRNA was determined using ISH methods previously described (11, 22, 23). Briefly, 20-μm coronal sections were collected on gelatin-coated slides and stored at –80 C. Sections were thawed, air-dried, and postfixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffer for 20 min. Sections were dehydrated and rehydrated through graded ethanols (3 min each) and exposed to 0.25% acetic anhydride in 0.1 M triethanolamine (pH 8) for 8 min. After graded dehydration (1 min each), sections were incubated with prehybridization buffer (containing 50% formamide, 5x SET (3 M NaCl, 0.05 M EDTA, 0.6 M Tris buffer), 0.2% sodium dodecyl sulfate, 5x Denhardt’s solution, 0.5 mg/ml salmon sperm sheared DNA, 250 μg/μl yeast tRNA, 100 mM dithiothreitol, and 10% dextran sulfate) in a humidified chamber for 1 h (at 55 C for the GR mRNA probe and at 42 C for the CRF mRNA probe). Hippocampal sections were hybridized overnight with 35S-labeled GR-RNA probe [originally from Dr. K. Yamamoto, courtesy of Dr. J. Masters (Pfizer, Inc., Ann Arbor, MI)] in hybridization buffer. Sections underwent a 30-min ribonuclease digestion at 37 C and were washed successively at 62 C in 2x standard saline citrate (SSC) and 1x SSC for 5 min each (1x SSC denotes 0.15 M NaCl and 15 mM trisodium citrate buffer, pH 7.0), in 0.25x SSC for 30 min, and in 0.1x SSC and 0.03x SSC for 1 h each. PVN sections were hybridized with 35S-labeled CRF DNA probe overnight and washed at 55 C in 2x SSC and 1x SSC for 5 min each, then successively in four fresh solutions of 0.3x SSC for 15 min each. All sections were dehydrated in graded ethanols containing 0.3 M ammonium acetate, followed by 100% ethanol, and apposed to film (Kodak BioMax MR film, MR-1; Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY) for 10–12 d.

    Semiquantitative analysis and statistical considerations

    Unbiased methods for section sampling and analyses were conducted without knowledge of treatment (18, 22, 23). GR mRNA ISH signal was analyzed on digitized films using the ImageTool software program (University of Texas Health Science Center, San Antonio, TX) as described previously (22). The linear range of ODs was evaluated using 14C standards. The mean ± SE of four anatomically matched sections was calculated for each brain. Statistical significance (P < 0.05) among groups was determined using one-factor ANOVA with Bonferonni’s post hoc test (PRISM, GraphPad, Inc.).

    Results

    Hippocampus-mediated learning and memory function is improved in nonhandled rats after early-life CRF1 antagonist treatment

    The Morris watermaze paradigm used here involves 2 d of training, followed by the testing day (d 3). As shown in Fig. 1A, on the first trial of d 3, nonhandled rats treated with CRF1 antagonist reached the platform’s new location faster than controls [between groups: F(3,40) = 4.51; P < 0.05] and with similar latencies as naive and CRF1 antagonist-treated, handled rats (P > 0.05). The nonhandled group needed the most time to reach the escape platform across trials on d 3 [between groups: F(3,5) = 35.03; P < 0.001]. The performance of nonhandled rats treated with CRF1 antagonist, did not differ from that of both naive and antagonist-treated handled rats. Figure 1B summarizes performance on all 3 d. The time needed for the antagonist-treated nonhandled rats to reach the platform on each day was significantly faster than that for controls [between groups: F(3,2) = 51.19; P < 0.001] and was indistinguishable from that for handled groups (P > 0.05).

    As shown in Fig. 2A, all groups learned to find the platform during the 2 d of training, i.e. less time required to reach the platform with subsequent trials [within group d 1: F(3,6) = 54.32; P < 0.001; d 2: F(3,6) = 42.80; P < 0.001], but to a markedly different degree [between groups d 1: F(3,6) = 9.80; P < 0.001; d 2: F(3,6) = 11.63; P < 0.001]. Nonhandled rats treated with CRF1 antagonist escaped faster than controls (P < 0.01), and their performance levels were similar to those of naive and CRF1 antagonist-treated, handled rats (P > 0.05). Vehicle administration did not influence spatial navigation at any point. The mean time (±SE) per trial was compared between groups using a paired t test: naive and vehicle-treated nonhandled rats, P = 0.53 (Fig. 2B); naive and vehicle-treated, handled rats, P = 0.28 (Fig. 2B). Therefore, data for naive and vehicle-treated groups were combined.

    Hippocampus-dependent retrieval memory was also tested using the object recognition paradigm (18, 21). Objects 1 and 2 were explored for similar amounts of time during the first day of the recognition test (Fig. 3A). The amount of time spent exploring object 1 did not differ between NH and NH+Veh rats (by t test, P = 0.76), and this was also true for object 2 (by t test, P = 0.92; Fig. 3A, upper panel). Therefore, data from naive and vehicle-treated nonhandled groups were combined. Similarly, the exploration time of object 1 (by t test, P = 0.95) and that of object 2 (by t test, P = 0.74) did not differ between H and H+Veh rats (Fig. 3A, middle panel), and these data were consolidated. The lower panel of Fig. 3A shows that on the first day of the test, the time spent exploring objects 1 and 2 did not differ within each group (by t tests: NH, P = 0.98; H, P = 0.73; NH+A, P = 0.73; H+A, P = 0.95). Similarly, the exploration time of each object was not significantly different among groups [by one-factor ANOVA: object 1: F(3,59) = 0.29; P = 0.83; object 2: F (3,59) = 0.24; P = 0.87].

    The second day of the object recognition test examined whether rats remembered the familiar object from the previous day. Indeed, all groups spent more time exploring the novel object and less time exploring the familiar object. However, the ratio of novel/familiar object exploration times differed significantly among groups: NH = 1.6 ± 0.06, H = 2.6 ± 0.27, NH+A = 2.5 ± 0.15; H+A = 2.65 ± 0.23 [one-factor ANOVA: F(3,59) = 8.77; P < 0.001; Fig. 3B]. Total exploration time did not vary among groups on either day of testing [d 1: F(3,59) = 0.33; P = 0.80; d 2: F(3,59) = 1.1; P = 0.37].

    CRF1 antagonist increases GR mRNA in nonhandled rats

    In accordance with previous findings (11, 15), levels of hippocampal GR mRNA were significantly up-regulated in handled rats compared with levels in nonhandled rats (Fig. 4). In comparison with nonhandled controls (naive and vehicle-treated), CRF1 antagonist treatment increased hippocampal GR mRNA expression in CA1 pyramidal cell layer [F(4,25) = 12.47; P < 0.001] and dentate gyrus granule cells [F(4,25) = 13.45; P < 0.001] to levels indistinguishable from those of naive and vehicle-treated handled animals (Fig. 4A). Administration of vehicle via osmotic pumps influenced neither hippocampal GR expression of nonhandled groups (by t tests: CA1 region, P = 0.71; dentate gyrus, P = 0.83) nor that of handled groups (by t tests: CA1 region, P = 0.66; dentate gyrus, P = 0.24). Treatment-induced augmentation of GR mRNA signal in hippocampus is depicted in bright-field photomicrographs derived from handled, nonhandled, vehicle-treated handled, and CRF1 antagonist-treated nonhandled rats (Fig. 4B).

    CRF expression in PVN is reduced in CRF1 antagonist-treated nonhandled rats

    CRF mRNA levels in PVN of adult rats handled early in life were lower compared with those in nonhandled controls [F(5,19) = 28.69; P < 0.001; Fig. 5]. Administration of CRF1 antagonist significantly down-regulated CRF expression in PVN of nonhandled rats (P < 0.01) to levels that did not differ significantly from those of naive or vehicle-treated handled rats. Expression of CRF mRNA in PVN was not influenced by vehicle administration in either handled or nonhandled groups (P > 0.05).

    Early-life treatment with CRF1 antagonist does not alter adult basal or 30 min poststress plasma CORT levels

    Administration of CRF1 antagonist early in life did not influence basal morning CORT levels [F(3,16) = 0.56; P = 0.64; Fig. 6]. A 20-min restraint stress elicited a significant elevation of plasma CORT 30 min after its onset (by t tests per group: NH, P < 0.001; H, P < 0.01; NH+Antag, P < 0.001, H+Antag, P < 0.001). One-factor ANOVA comparing mean (±SE) CORT levels among the groups suggested that at 30 min after restraint onset, plasma levels of CORT were similar in CRF1 antagonist-treated handled rats and handled controls (P > 0.05). Levels in both of these groups were significantly lower than those in nonhandled groups [F(3,15) = 30.02; P < 0.001; Fig. 6]. The CRF1 antagonist treatment on P10–17 did not change plasma CORT levels 30 min after stress onset in nonhandled rats (P > 0.05). Adult adrenal weights did not differ among groups [F(3,18) = 0.30; P = 0.82].

    Discussion

    The principal findings of the current study are that administration of a selective CRF1-antagonist, NBI 30775, to nonhandled rats from P10–17 1) improves performance on hippocampus-dependent learning and memory tests, 2) increases hippocampal GR mRNA levels and 3) reduces CRF expression in the PVN. Together, these antagonist-evoked changes lead to learning/memory performances and gene expression levels in adult nonhandled rats that resemble those in rats handled early in life.

    Adult rats handled early in life perform better in spatial learning tasks, such as the Morris watermaze (14). The current study confirms these findings and shows that CRF1 antagonist treatment to nonhandled rats also facilitates spatial learning and memory. Nonhandled rats treated with CRF1 antagonist required less time to find the escape platform during the 2 training days compared than those in naive and vehicle-treated nonhandled groups. In addition, performance was improved on d 3 of testing, when the hidden platform was placed in a new quadrant in the watermaze, and the ability to apply the previously learned search strategies to find the new platform location using the same spatial cues was tested. On the first trial of d 3, all rats initially swam to the quadrant where the escape platform was located during training. Rats treated with CRF1 antagonist found the hidden platform’s new location in less time than nonhandled control groups, with latencies similar to those of handled groups. Because the performance on this series of tests requires an intact hippocampus (20), these findings support a long-lasting influence of early-life handling on hippocampal function and suggest that treatment with NBI 30775 influences hippocampal function of nonhandled rats. (It should be noted that antagonist administration on P10–17 did not influence the learning/memory function of handled rats.)

    CRF1 antagonist treatment may improve spatial learning and memory via several potential mechanisms: For example, spatial learning in the watermaze is particularly sensitive to CORT levels, and both excessive or suboptimal amounts may lead to learning and memory impairments (24, 25). As shown here and in previous studies (11, 15), CORT levels after restraint stress are lower in adult rats handled early in life compared with nonhandled cohorts. However, early-life treatment of CRF1 antagonist to nonhandled rats did not reduce plasma CORT levels 30 min after the onset of restraint stress. Because this single time point may not reflect that overall profile of the hormonal stress response of these rats, it is difficult to determine from these data whether alterations in stress-evoked plasma CORT levels account for the improved watermaze performance found in this group.

    CORT exerts its effects on hippocampal neurons via activation of receptors, including GR (26). Extensive previous work has demonstrated that GR expression levels are increased in adult rats handled early in life (11, 15). Indeed, the current study finds that CRF1 antagonist treatment increased GR expression in nonhandled rats to levels typical of those in rats handled early in life, supporting the idea that increased GR levels play a key role in regulating the stress responses of handled rats (6, 8, 11); in the presence of similar levels of plasma CORT, higher availability of GR may influence the efficiency of the actions triggered by CORT on hippocampal neurons (27, 28). In this study plasma CORT levels of CRF1 antagonist-treated nonhandled rats 30 min after stress onset were higher than those of handled rats. This may be attributable to the fact that the single time point may not be representative of the complete profile of the hormonal stress response. Future studies should further investigate this point.

    It might be argued that the Morris watermaze paradigm involves adverse situations, so that potential subtle changes in the coping abilities of the experimental groups (handled, nonhandled, and antagonist treated) may influence the results. To control for these possibilities, hippocampus-dependent learning and memory were also examined using an independent, comparatively less stressful, object recognition test. Rats were habituated in the testing room and cage for 15 min, 24 h before the first day of testing, to minimize novelty stress. On the second day of testing, memory of the familiar object was significantly improved in the CRF1 antagonist-treated group, i.e. more time was spent exploring the novel object, resembling exploration times of early-life-handled cohorts. These data support the idea that the enhanced memory function of the nonhandled, CRF1 antagonist-treated group is independently regulated from their peripheral CORT responses 30 min after stress.

    An alternative mechanism by which antagonist administration post hoc to nonhandled rats might lead to learning and memory improvements is a potential influence on endogenous CRF. In addition to its action on peripheral CRF receptors, when administered as a bolus at the doses used in this study, CRF1 antagonist crosses the blood-brain barrier and partially blocks central CRF1 receptor sites (17). CRF1 is found in many brain regions involved in learning and memory function, e.g. the frontal cortex and amygdala, as well as in the hippocampus (19, 29), the key substrate of the memory functions tested here. Recent research has implicated hippocampal CRF (30) in learning and memory (4, 18, 31). During the time of continuous CRF1 antagonist infusion, from P10–17, the number of CRF-immunoreactive neurons in the CA1 and CA3 hippocampal stratum pyramidal region increases by approximately 300% (from P11–18) (32). In addition, CRF1 receptor levels in hippocampus evolve with age (33). The effects of the endogenous ligand, CRF, on hippocampal CRF1 receptor expression have been demonstrated during the ages studied here (19). It is also conceivable that partially blocking CRF1 receptors from P10–17 may play a role in modulating cognitive function, either directly or via alterations in hippocampal CRF levels. These possibilities merit further investigation.

    The mechanisms by which selective CRF1 antagonist treatment led to persisting increases in hippocampal GR expression are not entirely clear. The findings of this study support the postulation that pituitary CRF1 receptors in handled rats are less activated starting on approximately P10, resulting in lower plasma CORT levels in response to intermittent stress during the critical developmental period during the first few weeks of life (11). Because hippocampal GR expression is largely governed by plasma CORT (34), a potential effect of reduced CORT spikes during stress (before P45) might be increased levels of hippocampal GR. Partially blocking CRF1 receptors on P10–17, as in this study in nonhandled rats, may recapitulate the lower plasma CORT levels at this age in handled rats, resulting in enduring increase in hippocampal GR.

    Increased levels of hippocampal GR are typically associated with a reduction of hypothalamic CRF levels (13, 15). These latter two changes were found after CRF1 antagonist treatment in the current study. However, the design of the study did not permit us to determine whether the relationship of hippocampal GR and hypothalamic CRF found here is causal.

    In summary, the enduring effects of enhanced early-life maternal care in handled rats on hippocampal GR gene expression, CRF mRNA in the PVN, and learning and memory are recapitulated by post hoc partial blockade of CRF1 receptor in nonhandled animals. These results may provide novel molecular targets influencing the long-lasting effects of early-life experience on learning and memory functions (35).

    Acknowledgments

    We thank Dr. Dimitri Grigoriadis for his kind gift of NBI 30775, and Michele Hinojosa for editorial assistance.

    Footnotes

    This work was supported by National Institutes of Health Grants 39307, 28912, and 07444.

    Abbreviations: CORT, Corticosterone; CRF, corticotropin-releasing factor; GR, glucocorticoid receptor; ISH, in situ hybridization histochemistry; P, postnatal day; PVN, paraventricular nucleus; SSC, standard saline citrate.

    References

    Ammerman RT, Cassisi JE, Hersen M, van Hassel VB 1986 Consequences of physical abuse and neglect in children. Clin Psychol Rev 6:291–310

    Bremner JD, Randall P, Vermetten E, Staib L, Bronen RA, Mazure C, Capelli S, McCarthy G, Innis RB, Charney DS 1997 Magnetic resonance imaging-based measurement of hippocampal volume in posttraumatic stress disorder related to childhood physical and sexual abuse—a preliminary report. Biol Psychiatry 41:23–32

    Sanchez MM, Ladd CO, Plotsky PM 2001 Early adverse experience as a developmental risk factor for later psychopathology: Evidence from rodent and primate models. Dev Psychopathol 13:419–449

    Avishai-Eliner S, Brunson KL, Sandman CA, Baram TZ 2002 Stressed out Or in (utero). Trends Neurosci 25:518–524

    Walker CD, Scribner KA, Cascio CS, Dallman MF 1991 The pituitary-adrenocortical system of neonatal rats is responsive to stress throughout development in a time-dependent and stressor-specific fashion. Endocrinology 128:1385–1395

    Liu D, Diorio J, Tannenbaum B, Caldji C, Francis D, Freedman A, Sharma S, Pearson D, Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ 1997 Maternal care, hippocampal glucocorticoid receptors, and hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal response to stress. Science 277:1659–1662

    Liu D, Diorio J, Day JC, Francis DD, Meaney MJ 2000 Maternal care, hippocampal synaptogenesis and cognitive development in rats. Nat Neurosci 3:799–806

    Bredy TW, Humpartzoomian RA, Cain DP, Meaney MJ 2003 Partial reversal of the effect of maternal care on cognitive function through environmental enrichment. Neuroscience 118:571–576

    Mirescu C, Peters JD, Gould E 2004 Early life experience alters response of adult neurogenesis to stress. Nature Neurosci 7:841–846

    Hess JL, Denenberg VH, Zarrow MX, Pfeifer D 1969 Modification of the corticosterone response curve as a function of handling in infancy. Physiol Behav 4:109–111

    Avishai-Eliner S, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Tabatchnik E, Brunson KL, Baram TZ 2001 Downregulation of hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing hormone messenger ribonucleic acid precedes early-life hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor-mRNA changes. Endocrinology 142:89–97

    Meerlo P, Horvath KM, Nagy GM, Bohus B, Koolhaas JM 1999 The influence of postnatal handling on adult neuroendocrine and behavioural stress reactivity. J Neuroendocrinol 11:925–933

    Meaney MJ, Aitken DH, van Berkel C, Bhatnagar S, Sapolsky RM 1988 Effect of neonatal handling on age-related impairments associated with the hippocampus. Science 239:766–768

    Huot RL, Plotsky PM, Lenox RH, McNamara RK 2002 Neonatal maternal separation reduces hippocampal mossy fiber density in adult Long Evans rats. Brain Res 950:52–63

    Plotsky PM, Meaney MJ 1993 Early, postnatal experience alters hypothalamic corticotropin-releasing factor (CRF) mRNA, median eminence CRF content and stress-induced release in adult rats. Mol Brain Res 18:195–200

    De Kloet ER, Oitzl MS, Joels M 1999 Stress and cognition: are corticosteroids good or bad guys Trends Neurosci 22:422–426

    Heinrichs SC, De Souza EB, Schulteis G, Lapsansky JL, Grigoriadis DE 2002 Brain penetrance, receptor occupancy and anti-stress in vivo efficacy of a small molecule corticotropin releasing factor type I receptor selective antagonist. Neuropsychopharmacology 27:194–202

    Brunson KL, Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Bender R, Chen Y, Baram TZ 2001 Long-term, progressive hippocampal cell loss and dysfunction induced by early-life administration of corticotropin-releasing hormone reproduce the effects of early-life stress. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98:8856–8861

    Brunson KL, Grigoriadis DR, Lorang MT, Baram TZ 2002 Corticotropin releasing hormone (CRH) downregulates the function of its receptor (CRF1) and induces CRF1 expression in hippocampal and cortical regions of the immature rat. Exp Neurol 176:75–86

    Morris R 1984 Developments of a water-maze procedure for studying spatial learning in the rat. J Neurosci Methods 11:47–60

    Clark RE, Zola SM, Squire LR 2000 Impaired recognition memory in rats after damage to the hippocampus. J Neurosci 20:8853–8860

    Eghbal-Ahmadi M, Avishai-Eliner S, Hatalski CG, Baram TZ 1999 Differential regulation of the expression of corticotropin-releasing factor receptor type 2 (CRF2) in the hypothalamus and amygdala of the immature rat by sensory input and food intake. J Neurosci 19:3982–3991

    Fenoglio KA, Avishai-Eliner S, Chen Y, Baram TZ 2004 Region-specific onset of handling-induced changes in corticotropin releasing hormone and glucocorticoid receptor expression. Endocrinology 145:121–136

    Oitzl MS, de Kloet RE 1992 Selective corticosteroid antagonist modulates specific aspects of spatial orientation learning. Behav Neurosci 106:62–71

    Oitzl MS, Fluttert M, Sutanto W, de Kloet ER 1998 Continuous blockade of brain glucocorticoid receptors facilitates spatial learning and memory in rats. Eur J Neurosci 10:3759–3766

    de Kloet RE, Vreugdenhil E, Oitzl MS, Joels M 1998 Brain corticosteroid receptor balance in health and disease. Endocr Rev 19:269–301

    Linthorst AC, Flachskamm C, Barden N, Holsboer F, Reul JM 2000 Glucocorticoid receptor impairment alters CNS responses to a psychological stressor: an in vivo microdialysis study in transgenic mice. Eur J Neurosci 12:283–291

    Cole TJ, Myles K, Purton JF, Brereton PS, Solomon NM, Godfrey DI, Funder JW 2001 GRKO mice express an aberrant dexamethasone-binding glucocorticoid receptor, but are profoundly glucocorticoid resistant. Mol Cell Endocrinol 173:193–202

    Chen Y, Brunson KL, Muller MB, Cariaga W, Baram TZ 2000 Immunocytochemical distribution of corticotropin-releasing hormone receptor type-1 (CRF(1))-like immunoreactivity in the mouse brain: light microscopy analysis using an antibody directed against the C-terminus. J Comp Neurol 420:305–323

    Chen Y, Bender RA, Brunson KL, Pomper JK, Grigoriadis DE, Wurst W, Baram TZ 2004 Modulation of dendritic differentiation by corticotropin-releasing factor in the developing hippocampus. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 101:15782–15787

    Blank T, Nijholt I, Grammatopoulos DK, Randeva HS, Hillhouse EW, Spiess J 2003 Corticotropin releasing factor receptors couple to multiple G-proteins to activate diverse intracellular signaling pathways in mouse hippocampus: role in neuronal excitability and associative learning. J Neurosci 23:700–707

    Chen Y, Bender RA, Frotscher M, Baram TZ 2001 Novel and transient populations of corticotropin-releasing hormone-expressing neurons in developing hippocampus suggest unique functional roles: a quantitative spatiotemporal analysis. J Neurosci 21:7171–7181

    Baram TZ, Hatalski CG 1998 Neuropeptide-mediated excitability: a key triggering mechanism for seizure generation in the developing brain. Trends Neurosci 21:471–476

    Herman JP, Spencer R 1998 Regulation of hippocampal glucocorticoid receptor gene transcription and protein expression in vivo. J Neurosci 18:7462–7473

    Kaufman J, Charney D 2001 Effects of early stress on brain structure and function: implications for understanding the relationship between child maltreatment and depression. Dev Psychopathol 13:451–471(Kristina A. Fenoglio, Kri)