当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《国际神经病学神经外科学杂志》 > 2004年第2期 > 正文
编号:11355468
Collateral brain damage, a potential source of cognitive impairment after selective surgery for control of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy
http://www.100md.com 《神经病学神经外科学杂志》
     1 Department of Epileptology, University Clinic Bonn, Bonn, Germany

    2 Department of Neurosurgery, University Clinic Bonn

    3 Department of Radiology/Neuroradiology, University Clinic Bonn

    Correspondence to:

    Dr C Helmstaedter

    University Clinic of Epileptology Bonn, Sigmund Freud Str 25, 53225 Bonn, Germany; Helmstaedter@uni-bonn.de

    ABSTRACT

    Background: Highly selective epilepsy surgery in temporal lobe epilepsy is intended to achieve seizure freedom at a lower cognitive risk than standard en bloc resections, but bears the risk of collateral cortical damage resulting from the surgical approach.

    Objective: To investigate cortical damage associated with selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy (SAH).

    Methods: 34 epileptic patients were evaluated. They were randomly assigned to SAH using either a sylvian (9 left/10 right) or a transcortical surgical approach (5 left/10 right). Postoperative MRI signal intensity changes adjacent to the approach were correlated with performance changes in serial word and design list learning.

    Results: Losses in verbal learning and recognition memory were positively related to signal intensity changes, independent of the side of the resection, the surgical approach, or the extent of the mesial resection. Losses in consolidation/retrieval (memory) were greater after left sided surgery. Losses in design learning were related to right sided surgery and signal intensity changes. Seizure outcome (85% seizure-free) did not differ depending on the side or type of surgery.

    Conclusions: Collateral damage to cortical tissues adjacent to the surgical approach contributes to postoperative verbal and figural memory outcome after SAH. Controlling for collateral damage may clarify the controversial memory outcomes after SAH reportedselective amygdalo-hippocampectomy; collateral damage; epilepsy; mem by different surgical centres.

    Keywords: selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy; collateral damage; epilepsy; memory

    Abbreviations: AVLT, auditory verbal learning test; SAH, selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy; FLAIR, fluid attenuated inversion recovery

    Despite advances in the pharmacological management of location related epilepsy, seizures remain refractory to medical treatment in approximately 15–30% of patients.1,2 For these patients epilepsy surgery can be a successful treatment option.3 Epilepsy surgery, however, bears an undeniable risk of causing additional cognitive impairment.4 Temporal lobe resections still represent the most frequent type of surgery and, accompanying progress in neuroimaging and electroencephalographic monitoring, there is a "new wave" in epilepsy surgery away from standard en bloc resections towards tailored and highly selective approaches.5,6 Restricting the resection to already damaged and epileptogenic tissue was originally thought to spare unaffected brain areas and their associated brain functions.5,6 However, there have been contradictory findings relating to memory loss after left selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy (SAH).7–10 The same is true of studies comparing the outcome of selective surgery versus standard two thirds anterior temporal lobectomy.6,11–13

    In this report we call attention to a factor that has not been considered so far in discussions on cognitive outcome after selective epilepsy surgery—collateral damage to brain tissue caused by the surgical approach. This turns out to be a potential source of postoperative cognitive impairment.6

    METHODS

    The study originally comprised 38 patients with left hemisphere language dominance who had been randomly assigned to selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy for mesial temporal lobe epilepsy, using either a transsylvian14 or a transcortical approach.15 Informed consent was obtained from the patients for randomisation and the study was approved by the ethics commission of the university clinics, Bonn. In our centre, SAH is generally indicated where there is a clinical diagnosis of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy and suspicion of mesial temporal sclerosis or atrophy as the sole pathology.

    Transsylvian amygdalo-hippocampectomies are done through a pterional craniotomy of approximately 5 cm in diameter. After microsurgical dissection of the sylvian fissure (2.5–3 cm), the temporal horn of the lateral ventricle is entered through the temporal stem, thus enabling resection of the mesial structures (amygdala, hippocampus, parahippocampus). The collateral sulcus is the intended lateral resection border, while the area of maximum brain stem diameter is the dorsal border.

    In transcortical amygdalo-hippocampectomies only the approach to the temporal horn is different: a 3 cm craniotomy is centred on the projection of the middle temporal gyrus. After a 2 cm corticotomy, the temporal horn is approached through the white matter with the aid of neuronavigation, followed by a hippocampal resection, as described above.

    Three patients after left amygdalo-hippocampectomy had to be excluded from the because their magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) results were not available for review. One patient developed a postoperative space occupying intra-axial haematoma after left amygdalo-hippocampectomy, which was removed surgically. This patient was excluded because of persisting dysphasia and mild hemiparesis. There was no mortality in the series. Minor complications which did not lead to exclusion were evident in two patients with signs of meningeal inflammation, one patient with an asymptomatic small choroidal infarction, and one patient with some blood in the resection cavity, which did not require specific treatment. Visual field defects (contralateral upper quadranopias), if present, were not encountered as complications. Thus 34 patients were evaluated for this study. Nineteen had a sylvian (nine left, 10 right) approach, and 15 a transcortical approach (five left, 10 right).

    Verbal learning and memory were assessed preoperatively and three months after surgery by parallel versions of a serial word list learning test, which requires learning and immediate recall of a word list (A) of 15 words over five trials, delayed recall after distraction (learning a second list (B) in one trial) and after a delay, and recognition of list A items from among alternatives (German pendant of the auditory verbal learning test (AVLT)16). The chosen measures of "learning over five trials," "loss of learned items in delayed free recall," and "recognition memory (correct minus errors)" differentially assess aspects of verbal short term/working memory and verbal long term consolidation/retrieval, and reflect more temporo-cortical or temporo-mesial functioning, respectively.17 Figural memory was assessed using a revised version of a serial design list learning test which requires learning and reproduction of nine abstract designs.18 Each design consists of five lines of equal length, and the designs have to be reproduced using five wooden sticks. Learning over four trials was chosen as the dependent measure. This test has been shown to be sensitive to right temporal lobe function and surgery.19,20

    High resolution MRI was undertaken a week after surgery, using a 1.5 Tesla system (Gyroscan ACS-NT, Philips Medical Systems, Best, Netherlands). Results of post-surgical MRI (axial and coronal FLAIR and T2 weighted fast spin echo images) were assessed by two independent examiners with respect to, first, the extent of hippocampal resection (defined negatively by measuring the distance from a line through the dorsal resection border to a line along the dorsal border of the quadrigeminal plate (fig 1A, 1B, left side); and second, the extent of post-surgical brain tissue signal changes along the transsylvian or transcortical path to the mesial temporal lobe ("collateral damage"), quantified by the mean diameter on coronal T2 weighted or FLAIR fast spin echo images (fig 1A, 1B, right side). "Collateral damage" was considered on the one hand as a continuous variable, ranging from 0 to 22 mm, and on the other as a dichotomous variable. Defining a diameter greater than the median (10 mm) as a significant lesion, 15 of the 34 patients were classified as showing greater collateral temporal cortical damage, and 19 as having lesser or no such damage.

    Figure 1 (A) T2 weighted fast spin echo images in axial (left) and coronal (right) orientation after left amygdalo-hippocampectomy (transsylvian approach). The axial slice shows the measurement of the extent of hippocampal resection (-11 mm). On the coronal slice only a small area of decent postoperative "damage" (9 mm in transverse diameter) can be seen adjacent to the sylvian fissure. (B) T2 weighted fast spin echo images in axial (left) and coronal (right) orientation after right transsylvian amygdalo-hippocampectomy with a much larger zone of postoperative alterations. The axial (left) image shows a similar extent of hippocampal resection (-10 mm), but note the hypointense signal alterations in the right lateral temporal lobe. The quantification of "collateral damage" results in a 22 mm zone adjacent to the sylvian fissure.

    Statistics

    Data were analysed by repeated measurement analysis of covariance (MANCOVA), which included preoperative/postoperative memory testing as the "within subject" factor, and side of surgery, surgical approach, and the diameter of the MRI signal intensity changes (dichotomous variable) as between subject factors. The extent of the mesial resection was controlled by covariance analysis. A prediction model of memory outcome (difference score: post-pre) was calculated by backward multiple regression analysis (p for removal <0.1). Regression analysis took into consideration baseline performance, side of surgery, surgical approach, seizure outcome, age, age at the onset of epilepsy, the degree of the mesial resection, and the degree of signal intensity changes (continuous variable).

    RESULTS

    The patient groups (transsylvian/transcortical) did not differ with respect to demographic variables (sex, age, intelligence quotient), clinical variables (age at onset, duration of epilepsy), seizure outcome (84% v 87% completely seizure-free), or the extent of the mesial resection (remaining hippocampus (mean (SD)), 1.8 (1.7) v 1.6 (1.8) cm). Surgical approaches were not associated with a different mean degree of "collateral cortical damage" (10 (7) mm v 9 (6) mm). With respect to verbal memory, multivariate analysis of variance revealed an effect of the MRI measure on postoperative memory change independent of the chosen approach (interaction effect: test repetition x collateral temporal damage, F = 3.4, p = 0.03). This effect was obtained only with respect to the more cortical aspects of verbal memory (learning/data acquisition, p<0.001; recognition memory, p = 0.05). In multivariate analysis of variance the observed interaction effect overshadowed the known effects of the side of surgery on verbal learning and memory. This effect became evident only in univariate analysis of variance and here only for verbal recognition memory (analysis of variance: F = 6.5, p = 0.01) (see also table 1).

    Table 1 Pre- and postoperative memory as a function of side of surgery and collateral damage

    Differences between the surgical approaches only marginally disfavoured the transcortical approach. Regression analysis showed that only the degree of signal intensity changes (t = 3.5, p<0.01) together with the preoperative baseline performance (t = 0.26, p<0.05) remained in the regression model and explained about 40% of the postoperative change in verbal learning (F = 9.6, p<0.01). The respective correlation between the degree of signal intensity changes and postoperative change in verbal learning is shown in fig 2. Performance in the more mesial function of verbal memory consolidation/retrieval (delayed free recall) was not affected by the degree of collateral damage, but in multiple regression analysis the expected effect of the side of surgery became evident (t = 2.3, p<0.05); according to this, greater losses were observed after left sided surgery. Together with the baseline performance (t = 5.9, p<0.001), the side of surgery explained 54% of the variance in performance changes in this measure (F = 18.1, p<0.001). Greater losses in recognition memory, which also reflects mesial and cortical aspects of memory, were observed after left sided resections (t = 3.6, p<0.01), with greater collateral damage (t = 3.0, p<0.01), with continuance of seizures (t = 2.6, p<0.05), and with better baseline performance (t = 2.0, p<0.1). Together, these variables explained 48% of the variance in recognition memory change (F = 6.3, p = 0.001).

    Figure 2 Regression showing a significant relation between the extent (diameter) of the collateral temporal cortical damage indicated by postoperative magnetic resonance imaging signal intensity changes and the postoperative change in verbal learning, independent of the surgical approach and the side of surgery (r = Pearson’s correlation coefficient, p = two tailed significance). Negative values on the y axis represent losses, positive values gains in the absolute number of words learned over five trials.

    With respect to figural memory, multivariate analysis of variance indicated a significant two way interaction (test repetition x side of surgery, F = 5.2, p = 0.03) and a significant three way interaction (test repetition x side of surgery x collateral damage, F = 5.9, p = 0.02). Patients with right sided surgery showed greater losses in design learning than patients with left sided surgery, particularly when there was evidence of collateral cortical damage (table 1). According to regression analysis, right sided surgery (t = 3.0, p = 0.005), better baseline performance (t = -2.0, p = 0.04), continuing seizures (t = 2.1, p = 0.04), and older age (t = 1.7, p = 0.09) were predictive of greater losses in design learning. Altogether the remaining variables explained 43% of variance.

    DISCUSSION

    Our results replicate the known specific effect of left sided selective surgery on verbal long term consolidation/retrieval, which appears to be associated with left temporo-mesial structures.10,11,17 Right sided surgery, in contrast, had a negative effect, particularly on design list learning. SAH in patients with mesial temporal lobe epilepsy with hippocampal sclerosis as the sole pathology was originally intended to spare the unaffected temporal cortex and associated functions. However, as in our recent study on SAH,10 we observed not only losses in verbal consolidation/retrieval but also in verbal learning and recognition memory. In addition a significant correlation between collateral cortical damage and changes in verbal learning and recognition was evident, indicating an effect of temporal cortical damage from the surgical approach, particularly on the short term and working memory aspects of verbal memory performance. The effect on verbal learning appeared to be non-specific, as it was observed independently of the side of surgery and the surgical approach (transsylvian v transcortical). This is consistent with findings that the short term and working memory aspects of verbal memory can be impaired non-specifically by lesions of different location and lateralisation if these affect receptive or executive components of memory processing.16 With design list learning we were never able to differentiate mesial from cortical damage qualitatively.19,20 However, the present data indicate that right sided temporal collateral cortical damage has a negative effect on this performance.

    We are aware that, in contrast to our previous studies on SAH,10,20 a rather short follow up interval was chosen in this study, so recovery from surgical damage can still be expected. The short interval was originally chosen in order to find out as quickly as possible whether a transcortical surgical approach has disadvantages over the transsylvian approach. The difference between the approaches was marginal and non-significant, although this may change with larger and more balanced groups. Furthermore it should be noted that more detailed information and more exact assessment of the collateral damage can be achieved using three dimensional MRI and MRI volumetry. Unfortunately digital datasets for three dimensional analyses were not available in the patients evaluated here.

    The decisive message of the present study, however, is that epilepsy surgery—even if stereotype resections are intended—is not a neuropsychological "black box" in the operational sense of the word. Damage caused by the surgical approach appears to be a potential source of postoperative memory impairment, which should be considered in the discussion of the advantages/disadvantages of different neurosurgical approaches, and we hope the present findings contribute to an understanding of the contradictory findings in previous reports.6–13

    REFERENCES

    Mattson RH, Cramer JA, Collins JF, et al. Comparison of carbamazepine, phenobarbital, phenytoin and primidone in partial and secondarily generalized tonic clonic seizures. N Engl J Med 1985;313:145–51.

    Kwan P, Brodie MJ. Early identification of refractory epilepsy. N Engl J Med 2000;342:314–319.

    Wiebe S, Blume WT, Girvin JP, et al. A randomized, controlled trial of surgery for temporal-lobe epilepsy. N Engl J Med 2001;345:311–18.

    Lee TM, Yip JT, Jones-Gotman M. Memory deficits after resection from left or right anterior temporal lobe in humans: a meta-analytic review. Epilepsia 2002;43:283–91.

    Stefan H, Halasz P, Gil-Nagel A, et al. Recent advances in the diagnosis and treatment of epilepsy. Eur J Neurol 2001;8:519–39.

    Clusmann H, Schramm J, Kral T, et al. Prognostic factors and outcome after different types of resection for temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurosurg 2002;97:1131–41.

    Wieser HG. Selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy for temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 1988;29:100–13.

    Goldstein LH, Polkey CE. Short-term cognitive changes after unilateral temporal lobectomy or unilateral amygdalo-hippocampectomy for the relief of temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neuosurg Psychiatry 1993;56:135–40.

    Regard M, Schiess R, Landis T. H?here Hirnfunktionen und Epilepsie – mit besonderer Berücksichtigung der Leistungen vor und nach Amygdalohippokampektomie. Z EEG-EMG 1996;27:257–62.

    Gleissner U, Helmstaedter C, Schramm J, et al. Memory outcome after selective amygdalohippocampectomy: a study in 140 patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia 2002;43:87–95.

    Helmstaedter C, Elger CE, Hufnagel A, et al. Different effects of left anterior temporal lobectomy, selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy, and temporal cortical lesionectomy on verbal learning, memory, and recognition. J Epilepsy 1996;9:39–45.

    Jones-Gotman M, Zatorre RJ, Olivier A, et al. Learning and retention of words and designs following excision from medial or lateral temporal-lobe structures. Neuropsychologia 1997;35:963–73.

    Pauli E, Pickel S, Schulemann H, et al. Neuropsychologic findings depending on the type of the resection in temporal lobe epilepsy. Adv Neurol 1999;81:373–377.

    Yasargil MG, Teddy PJ, Roth P. Selective amygdalo-hippocampectomy. Operative anatomy and surgical technique. Adv Tech Stand Neurosurg 1985;12:93–123.

    Olivier A. Transcortical selective amygdalohippocampectomy in temporal lobe epilepsy. Can J Neurol Sci 2000;27 (suppl 1) :68–76.

    Helmstaedter C, Lendt M, Lux S. VLMT Verbaler Lern- und Merkf?higkeitstest G?ttingen: Beltz Test GmbH, 2001:34–9.

    Helmstaedter C, Lehnertz K, Grunwald Th, et al. Differential involvement of left temporo-lateral and temporo-mesial structures in verbal declarative learning and memory: evidence from temporal lobe epilepsy. Brain Cognition 1997;35:110–31.

    Lamberti G. DCS: a visual learning and memory test for neuropsychological assessment G?ttingen: Hogrefe and Huber, 1999.

    Helmstaedter C, Pohl C, Hufnagel A, et al. Visual learning deficits in nonresected patients with right temporal lobe epilepsy. Cortex 1991;27:547–55.

    Gleissner U, Helmstaedter C, Elger CE. Right hippocampal contribution to visual memory: a presurgical and postsurgical study in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1998;65:665–9.(C Helmstaedter1, D Van Ro)