当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《英国医生杂志》 > 2005年第18期 > 正文
编号:11384470
Surveying the literature from animal experiments
http://www.100md.com 《英国医生杂志》
     Critical reviews may be helpful—not systematic ones

    The value of animal research for finding new treatments for human diseases is a continuing debate. The starting point of the debate must be the recognition of the past contributions of animal experiments to our understanding of disease and existing treatments. We can cite the major impact of research based on animals in diseases such as polio, kidney transplantation, and Parkinson's disease. Almost every form of conventional medical treatment (including most drugs, surgical treatments, and vaccines) was developed with the help of animal research.1-3 Most of what we know about the basic workings of the body—in humans and animals—has come to us through two centuries of animal experiments. Each decade of animal research has brought newer and deeper understanding.4 What we lack, however, are better methods of surveying the literature on animal experiments.

    Curiosity about fundamental biological mechanisms has yielded a rich harvest of useful knowledge. Although around 30% of current animal research is categorised as "fundamental" by the Home Office,3 much of this targets specific diseases. How do we know when the information gained from animal experiments is strictly relevant for the planning of clinical trials of new drugs?

    It might seem straightforward to ensure that, before a clinical trial of a new treatment commences, all relevant results from animal studies are systematically reviewed for evidence of safety and efficacy. Perhaps the best known case is that of the calcium channel blocker nimodipine as a potential neuroprotective agent after stroke. Some authors have claimed that animal experiments failed to prevent the problems that occurred in the clinical trials.5 6 But animal experiments did reveal the deleterious effects of this drug, and these results were published. The clinical trials, however, went ahead despite evidence from animal experiments that suggested caution. Why? What are the pressures (scientific, commercial, and others) that allow trials to progress even when the evidence is not compelling or even ambiguous? And what are the requirements to weigh all available evidence in balance rather than select the data that support the personal or economic imperative? Although the example of nimodipine is well known, other powerful recent examples of animal research informing medical advance also exist—for example, the recent development of a vaccine for the severe acute respiratory syndrome.7

    We need better methods of surveying the literature on animal experiments. The huge year on year increase in the numbers of studies reported makes it ever more likely that vital pieces of evidence go undetected. However, the proposal that systematic reviews of animal based research might solve this problem has two fundamental problems. Firstly, no mechanism exists for so called negative results to be published. Thus the absence of evidence for a particular drug action must often be inferred. This is not just an issue of publication bias; it is intrinsic to the experimental process. Scientific experiments are designed to test for evidence in favour of a particular experimental hypothesis and to abandon it if insufficient evidence is acquired.

    Secondly, the style of clinical trials and of animal research have important generic differences. Clinical trials of putative treatments entail testing the treatment on a cohort of sick humans. The design can vary, but the subjects can be quite similar from one trial to another, and this obviously facilitates meta-analysis and makes systematic review feasible. Pre-clinical animal trials entail testing specific effects on particular measures of physiological function while seeking to control all other possible variables. Ethical imperatives limit the number of animals used to the minimum and require that previously published studies are not simply repeated. Moreover, because of the systematic nature of the research, each experiment necessarily differs in its precise design, method, and dependent variables from those that have gone before making it much more difficult to combine data from different studies.

    What we need is critical review, rather than systematic review, of all the evidence before human trials commence. A critical review compiles and evaluates the different sources of experimental evidence on a qualitative basis. A difficulty with systematic reviews is that attempts to meet precise inclusion criteria often mean useful information is excluded. The reliability and validity of each animal model needs to be assessed on its merits and its relevance to the particular clinical application. While seeking to identify and protect against major problems at the early stage of development, no model is perfect and may still miss effects that are rare or species specific, and which can be revealed only in subsequent human trials of the new treatment. Partial information, while never perfect, is better than no information.

    Finally, the close association of basic and clinical science is an essential requirement for successful translation. This must include a critical appreciation of what experimental science has to offer in terms of a solution to the clinical problem.

    Roger Lemon, professor

    Institute of Neurology, London WC1N 3BG (rlemon@ion.ucl.ac.uk)

    Stephen B Dunnett, professor

    School of Biosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF10 3US

    Competing interests: None declared.

    References

    Royal Society. The use of non-human animals in research: a guide for scientists. London: Royal Society, 2004. www.royalsoc.ac.uk/document.asp?tip=0&id=1351 (accessed 30 Mar 2005).

    Academy of Medical Sciences. Restoring neurological function. Putting the neurosciences to work in neurorehabilitation. London: Academy of Medical Sciences, 2004. www.acmedsci.ac.uk/p_neurofunc.pdf (accessed 30 Mar 2005).

    House of Lords Select Committee on Animals in Scientific Procedures. HL Paper 150-1. London: Stationery Office, 2002. www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/ld200102/ldselect/ldanimal/150/150.pdf (accessed 13 Apr 2005).

    Greaves P, Williams A, Eve M. First dose of potential new medicines to humans: how animals help. Nature Drug Discovery 2004;3: 226-36.

    Pound P, Ebrahim S, Sandercock P, Bracken MB, Roberts I. Where is the evidence that animal research benefits humans? BMJ 2004;328: 514-7.

    Horn J, de Haan RJ, Vermeulen M, Luiten PGM, Limburg M. Nimodipine in animal model experiments of focal cerebral ischemia: a systematic review. Stroke 2001;32: 2433-8.

    Yang ZY, Kong WP, Huang Y, Roberts A, Murphy BR, Subbarao K, Nabel GJ. A DNA vaccine induces SARS coronavirus neutralization and protective immunity in mice. Nature 2004;428: 561-4.