当前位置: 首页 > 医学版 > 期刊论文 > 内科学 > 内分泌学杂志 > 2005年 > 第3期 > 正文
编号:11168265
Unexpected Effects of Perinatal Gonadal Hormone Manipulations on Sexual Differentiation of the Extrahypothalamic Arginine-Vasopressin System
     Program in Neuroscience and Department of Psychology (J.S.L.), Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48823; and Center for Neuroendocrine Studies (B.D.R., G.J.D.), University of Massachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts 01003

    Address all correspondence and requests for reprints to: Program in Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, Giltner Hall, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan 48823. E-mail: lonstein@msu.edu.

    Abstract

    The sexually dimorphic extrahypothalamic arginine-vasopressin (AVP) projections from the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis to the lateral septum (LS) and lateral habenula (LHb) are denser in males than females and, in rats, require males’ perinatal exposure to gonadal hormones but the absence of such exposure in females. We examined perinatal hormone effects on development of this sex difference in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), which show atypical effects of hormones on sexual differentiation of some reproductive behaviors. Neonatal castration reduced the number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and AVP immunoreactivity (ir) in the LS and LHb. Surprisingly, daily injections of 1000 μg of testosterone propionate (TP) during the first postnatal week did not maintain high levels of AVP-ir in neonatally castrated males. Furthermore, perinatal treatments with TP (75, 500, or 1000 μg), testosterone (100 μg), or dihydrotestosterone (200 μg) did not masculinize AVP-ir in the female LS or LHb. In fact, 1000 μg TP reduced it in some cases. However, 1000 μg TP lengthened anogenital distance, indicating that TP was biologically active. Neonatal estrogen receptor antagonism with tamoxifen reduced AVP-ir in the male LS, whereas treating neonatal females with the synthetic estrogen diethylstilbestrol increased septal AVP-ir. Tamoxifen and diethylstilbestrol had no effects in the LHb. Similar to rats, therefore, postnatal estrogen influences some components of the extrahypothalamic AVP system in prairie voles, but this developing system appears to be insensitive to exogenous androgens, including aromatizable androgens. Such insensitivity is atypical for a sexually dimorphic neural system in a rodent and may reflect the unusual effects of hormones on sexual differentiation of some behaviors in prairie voles.

    Introduction

    EXTRAHYPOTHALAMIC EXPRESSION OF the neuropeptide arginine-vasopressin (AVP) or its nonmammalian analog arginine-vasotocin is sexually dimorphic in many vertebrate species, with males showing markedly greater AVP or arginine-vasotocin expression than females (1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6). In rats, this sexually dimorphic system originates in AVP-synthesizing cells of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BST) and medial amygdala (MA) and terminates densely in the lateral septum (LS) and lateral habenula (LHb) (7, 8). Similar to almost all sex differences in the mammalian nervous system (9, 10, 11), differential exposure of developing males and females to gonadal hormones influences sex differences in extrahypothalamic AVP expression. For example, neonatal castration of male rats prevents the masculinization of AVP expression, whereas treating neonatal females with testosterone propionate (TP) masculinizes it (12, 13, 14). TP apparently requires aromatization into estradiol for much of its effect (14), although the reduced androgen dihydrotestosterone (DHT) can also partly masculinize this system in rats if given in high doses (14).

    As with many characteristics of the rodent brain, the influence of perinatal gonadal hormones in the generation of sex differences in extrahypothalamic AVP expression has been best studied in the laboratory rat. This is true even though sex differences in extrahypothalamic AVP expression exist in other rodent species (1, 15, 16, 17, 18, 19). It is virtually unknown in any other species whether sexual differentiation of this system relies on perinatal gonadal hormones in a manner similar to that in rats. It is quite possible that it does not because the mechanisms controlling sexual differentiation of other processes, including reproductive behaviors, can differ between species (20).

    The prairie vole (Microtus ochrogaster) is an arvicoline rodent that is unusual among mammals because it shows many indices of monogamy, including pair bonding with mates and biparental behavior (21). They are a unique model to study sexual differentiation of behavior because some studies demonstrate that exposure to gonadal hormones during development influences their later sexual and parental behaviors differently than it does in rats. Unlike rats (22), neonatal castration does not demasculinize sexual behavior in male prairie voles and, in some cases, neonatal TP does not masculinize it in females (23, 24). Furthermore, neonatal castration demasculinizes parental responsiveness in virgin male prairie voles, but perinatal treatment of virgin females with TP does not masculinize it (25, 26). The influence of gonadal hormones on the development of any sex difference in the prairie vole brain has not been examined and may differ from rats. The present studies investigated perinatal hormone effects on the development of sexually dimorphic AVP expression.

    Materials and Methods

    Subjects

    Subjects were F4 and F5 generation prairie voles (M. ochrogaster) that were born and raised in our colonies, which were established in 1996 at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst, and in 2002 at Michigan State University, from breeding stock originating from offspring of voles captured in 1994 from Urbana, IL, provided by Dr. Betty McGuire (Smith College, Northampton, MA) and Dr. Zuoxin Wang (Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL). These stocks were outbred most recently in 2000, with voles of Illinois origin provided by Dr. C. Sue Carter (University of Illinois at Chicago, Chicago, IL). Animals were mated by socially isolating adult virgin female and male prairie voles for 3 d, after which females were placed in the cage of an unfamiliar male. Animals were maintained on a 14-h light, 10-h dark cycle with an ambient temperature of 21 C. At all ages, animals were housed in plastic cages (48 x 28 x 16 cm) containing wood chips, wood shavings, and a substantial hay covering. Water and a food mixture containing cracked corn, whole oats, sunflower seeds, and Purina rabbit chow (ratio of 1:1:2:2) were available ad libitum.

    In experiments in which pups were manipulated prenatally via injections given to their dam (experiments 2 and 4), litters were fostered on the day of birth to unmanipulated surrogate lactating prairie voles from our colony that had given birth to a litter within the previous 24 h. Similar to our previous reports (25, 26), surrogate dams readily accepted foster pups, and the fostered pups in all cases had very high survival rates. Also similar to these previous reports, survival of neonatally manipulated pups raised by their own dams was invariably 100% in all experiments. All pups in a given litter received the same treatment. Subjects were weaned at 21 d of age and housed with their littermates in mixed-sex groups. When subjects were 30 d old, they were rehoused in single-sex groups of two to four animals per cage. Subjects remained housed with their same-sex littermates until killed at approximately 90 d old. Body weight and anogenital distance were measured either at the time of adult gonadectomy or at death and were consistent within each experiment. One and, in a few cases, two pups within a given litter were used as subjects. All procedures were performed in accord with the acceptable standards for use of animals in research by the National Institutes of Health, the University of Massachusetts, and Michigan State University.

    Adult gonadectomy and testosterone (T) capsule implantation

    Perinatal treatment of pups is described in detail for each experiment later in this article. In all cases, when pups reached 70–75 d of age, they were anesthetized with an ip injection of ketamine (62.5 mg/kg), xylazine (7.5 mg/kg), and acepromazine (0.8 mg/kg), and gonadally intact males and females were gonadectomized. Neonatally castrated males received a sham surgery. All subjects then received a single 2.5-cm SILASTIC brand capsule (Dow Corning, Midland, MI) filled with crystalline T implanted sc in the nape of their neck. Circulating levels of T in adult voles after implantation of a 2.5-cm long capsule have not been determined but are likely supraphysiological. The size of this capsule is consistent with the only other studies on the extrahypothalamic AVP system in prairie voles and is capable of maintaining normal extrahypothalamic AVP expression in adult males (27).

    Killing

    Subjects in all experiments were killed 3 wk after implantation of T-filled capsules. Subjects whose brains were used for immunocytochemistry were overdosed with ketamine and xylazine and perfused through the heart with 50 ml of 0.9% saline followed by 50 ml of 5% acrolein in sodium phosphate buffer (PB). Brains were postfixed overnight in 5% acrolein in PB and then submerged in a 30% sucrose-PB solution for 3 d until sectioning. Subjects whose brains were used for in situ hybridization analysis were rendered unconscious with carbon dioxide and decapitated. Brains were rapidly removed from the skull, flash-frozen in cold 2-methyl butane, and stored at –70 C until sectioning.

    Immunocytochemistry

    Brains were cut into 40-μm sections with a freezing microtome, and immunocytochemistry for AVP was performed on every third section throughout the entire brain as described previously (29). Sections were rinsed between each step three times for 5 min in Tris-buffered saline (TBS), incubated in 1% sodium borohydride for 10 min, 1% Triton X-100 and 1% hydrogen peroxide in TBS for 10 min, 20% normal goat serum in 0.3% Triton X-100 in TBS for 10 min, and rabbit anti-AVP primary antibody (1:15,000; ICN Biochemicals, Aurora, OH) at room temperature for approximately 18 h. Sections were then rinsed three times with 0.3% Triton X-100 in TBS, incubated in a biotinylated goat antirabbit secondary antibody (1:500; Vector Laboratories, Burlingame, CA), rinsed three times in TBS for 5 min each, and incubated with avidin-biotin complex (Vectastain Elite; Vector Laboratories) for 60 min. Visualization of AVP immunoreactivity (ir) occurred with a solution of 0.05% 3–3'-diaminobenzedine tetrahydrochloride, 0.04% ammonium chloride, 0.0004% glucose oxidase, 0.75 mM nickel ammonium sulfate, and 0.3% ?-D-glucose. Sections were mounted on microscope slides, dehydrated, and coverslipped.

    In situ hybridization

    Brains were thawed to approximately –17 C, and the entire BST and MA were cut into 20-μm sections on a cryostat. Sections were thaw-mounted onto Superfrost Plus slides (Fisher Scientific, Pittsburgh, PA) and stored at –80 C until further processing. The in situ hybridization was performed similarly to that described previously in the De Vries laboratory (14). Plasmids containing the cDNA AVP sequence (pGEM3-AVPexCc) were obtained from Dr. Tom Sherman (Georgetown University, Washington, D.C.), and the vector was cut using EcoRI and transcribed with SP6 RNA polymerase and UTP. The riboprobe contained 293 bases that complemented the rat AVP mRNA. The probe was labeled at the 3'-end with [35S]dATP (NEN Life Science Products, Products, Boston, MA) using terminal deoxynucleotidyl transferase (Life Technologies Inc., Rockville, MD). Every third section through the BST and MA was processed. Slides were air dried at room temperature, fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS for 30 min, rinsed in PBS, and incubated for 10 min in a solution of 1.5% triethanolamine hydrochloride-0.9% sodium chloride (SSC)-0.25% acetic anhydride (pH 8.0). Slides were then rinsed for 2 min in 0.03 M sodium citrate in 0.3 M SSC. Slides were incubated in 70, 80, 95, and 100% ethanol for 2 min each, incubated in chloroform for 5 min, incubated again in 100 and 95% ethanol for 5 min each, and air dried.

    Each slide was placed horizontally after applying 100 μl of the hybridization buffer (2,000,000 cpm/100 μl) consisting of 50% formamide, 10% dextan sulfate, 1 M Denhart’s solution, 2x SSC, 0.5 mg/ml yeast tRNA, and 0.1% sodium pyrophosphate in diethylpyrocarbonate-treated water. Slides were coverslipped with Parafilm (purchased from Fisher Scientific) and placed in a humidified incubator set at 37 C for approximately 14 h. Posthybridization washes consisted of 1x SSC (4 x 10 min each), 2x SSC in 50% formamide (2x 20 min each at 52 C), 2x SSC (2 x 5 min each), RNase buffer (10 min at 37 C), RNase buffer with 100 μg/ml RNase A (30 min at 37 C), 2x SSC (2 x 10 min each), 2x SSC in 50% formamide (2x 20 min at 52 C), 1x SSC (2 x 5 min each), and 70% ethanol (2 x 5 min each); and slides were then air dried. Slides were hybrized and coated with emulsion (NTB3, Kodak) and developed and fixed 2 wk later. Sections were then lightly counterstained with methyl green, dried, and coverslipped with glass microscope slides.

    Analysis of AVP fibers and cells

    All slides were randomized and coded for analysis. Density of AVP-ir fibers in the LS, LHb, and, in experiment 1, the paraventricular thalamic nucleus (PVT) was examined in sections of randomized and coded slides as described previously (29) with computerized image analysis using the NIH IMAGE 1.44 software (National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD). Fiber density was analyzed by gray-level thresholding and was expressed as the total area (in pixels) within a sample region covered by AVP-ir fibers. Measurements were standardized across brain sections by keeping the mean OD of the background constant. AVP-ir fiber density in the LS and LHb was measured bilaterally in an area of 450 x 350 μm from two sections per site per subject that had the highest fiber density; three sections per subject were analyzed bilaterally in the PVT as a control site in experiment 1. Within the LS, the area analyzed included the ventrolateral LS approximately at the level immediately rostral to the decussation of the anterior commissure, an area which covers the densest part of the AVP fiber plexus in this region and corresponds approximately to plate 18 of Paxinos and Watson’s atlas of the rat brain (30). In the LHb, the area of analysis included a caudal LHb region that has the densest AVP fiber plexus, corresponding approximately to plate 33 of Paxinos and Watson (30). Sections chosen for analysis of the PVT included the same sections that were analyzed for the LHb plus the next section lying immediately caudal.

    The total number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in six hemisections through the BST and MA were quantified by eye under bright-field illumination by a single observer (J.S.L.) unaware of subject condition. Grain density was determined by computerized gray-level thresholding in the four hemisections that contained the most AVP mRNA-expressing cells and analyzed with NIH IMAGE 1.44. From these four hemisections, 10 cells within the BST of each subject were randomly chosen for analysis, along with five cells from the MA. Grain density was expressed as the number of pixels that each cell had covered by grains.

    Data analyses

    Immunocytochemical data from experiments 1, 3, and 5–7 were analyzed with one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests. In situ hybridization data from experiment 2 were also analyzed with a one-way ANOVA followed by Bonferroni post hoc tests. Immunocytochemical data from experiment 4 were analyzed with a one-way ANOVA with control males included and then a two-way ANOVA including data only from females using prenatal and postnatal treatments as factors, with both of the ANOVAs followed by Bonferroni post hoc analysis. Data from males in experiment 8 were analyzed with an unpaired t test; data from females in experiment 8 failed the assumption of homogeneity of variance and were, instead, analyzed with a Mann-Whitney U test. Statistical significance was indicated by P 0.05.

    Results

    Experiment 1: effects of neonatal castration on AVP-ir

    Methods.

    Neonatal males were cryoanesthetized on ice within 2–12 h after birth. One group of males (n = 11) received two lateral incisions of their ventrum, and their testes were removed. The other half of the males received a sham castration (n = 8). A group of control neonatal females (n = 11) were also cryoanesthetized and received a sham surgery. Incisions were closed with silk suture. Pups were placed under a warm lamp until they were active, after which, they were returned to their dam. When 70–75 d old, subjects were gonadectomized (or received a sham surgery), implanted with a T-filled SILASTIC brand capsule, and killed 3 wk later as detailed above. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry as detailed above.

    Results.

    One neonatal sham male from experiment 1 was found to be an outlier in the amount of AVP-ir in the LS (Dixon’s outlier test, P 0.01) and was removed from the analysis for that site. Control males had significantly more AVP-ir in the LS than control females, and neonatally castrated males had levels of AVP-ir in the LS that were intermediate between the control males and females (F(2,27) = 9.19, P 0.001), with all groups significantly different from each other, (P 0.05; Figs. 1 and 2). A similar pattern of differences between groups was also found in the LHb (F(2,28) = 7.71, P 0.003; Figs. 1 and 2).

    FIG. 1. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM pixels covered by immunoreactive fibers) in the LS and LHb of female prairie voles and male prairie voles that received a neonatal sham surgery or neonatal castration. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups within each brain region; P 0.05.

    FIG. 2. Photomicrograph of AVP-ir in the LS of a representative control female (A), neonatally castrated male (B), and sham-operated control male (C) (right). LV, Lateral ventricle.

    Experiment 2: effects of perinatal hormone manipulations on AVP mRNA expression in males

    Methods.

    Pregnant females (n = 13) received a daily sc injection of 5 mg of the androgen receptor blocker flutamide (,,-trifluro-2-methyl-4-nitro-m-propoinotoluidide; Sigma, St. Louis, MO) dissolved in 100 μl sesame oil immediately followed by a sc injection of 1 mg of the aromatase inhibitor 1,4,6-androstatriene-3,17-dione (ATD; Steraloids Inc., Wilton, NH) dissolved in 100 μl sesame oil beginning 1 wk after pairing with a male. To generate control subjects, other pregnant females (n = 13) received two injections of 100 μl of sesame oil each day. The gestation period in prairie voles is similar to rats, so prenatal treatments were intended to cover at least the last 2 wk of the 3-wk gestation period. Our previous studies show that parturition occurs approximately 15 d after the beginning of the dams’ treatment with either ATD, flutamide, or sesame oil (25, 26). Doses were chosen based on the effectiveness of similar or even smaller doses to influence sexual differentiation in rats (for references, see Ref.28). Within 12 h after birth, half of the males from each group were castrated, and the other half received a sham surgery as described earlier. All pups from all groups were then fostered to recently parturient surrogate lactating dams from our colony. When 70–75 d old, all subjects were gonadectomized (or received a sham surgery if neonatally castrated), implanted with a T-filled capsule, and killed 3 wk later. Brains were processed for in situ hybridization as described earlier. The sample sizes for the male groups were as follows: prenatal oil/neonatal sham, n = 8; prenatal oil/neonatal castration, n = 7; prenatal ATD + flutamide/neonatal sham, n = 7; and prenatal ATD + flutamide/neonatal castration, n = 8. A group of neonatal females that was cryoanesthetized and received a sham surgery (n = 6), followed by ovariectomy and T capsule implant during adulthood, was also included in the study.

    Results.

    We were unable to collect data from the BST of one female, both the BST and MA of one sham male, and the BST of one neonatally castrated male. Another neonatally castrated male was found to be an outlier in the number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the BST (Dixon’s outlier test, P 0.01) and was removed from data analyses for this site. There were no significant effects of prenatal treatment with flutamide plus ATD on any measure, and groups of males were, therefore, collapsed across prenatal treatment for further analysis.

    There were significant differences between groups in the number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the BST (F(2,32) = 5.85, P 0.007; Figs. 3 and 4). Sham-castrated males had the greatest number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells, which was significantly greater than both neonatally castrated males and control females, which were statistically similar to each other. There were no significant differences between groups in the number of grains per cell within the BST (F(2,32) = 1.50, P 0.2; data not shown).

    FIG. 3. Number (mean ± SEM) of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the BST and MA of female prairie voles and male prairie voles that received either a neonatal sham surgery or neonatal castration. Different letters above bars indicate significant difference between groups within each brain region; P 0.05.

    FIG. 4. Photomicrographs of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the BST of a representative sham-operated control male, neonatally gonadectomized (GDX) male, and sham-operated control female. LV, Lateral ventricle.

    Within the MA, there were no significant differences between any of the groups in the number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells (F(2,34) = 0.18, P 0.8; Fig. 3) or in the number of grains per cell (F(2,34) = 1.41, P 0.2; data not shown).

    Experiment 3: effects of T replacement on AVP-ir in neonatally castrated males

    Methods.

    Neonatal males were cryoanesthetized and received either a sham surgery or were castrated. Immediately after surgery and continuing once a day for the next 6 d, sham subjects (n = 8) received a sc injection of 100 μl sesame oil, and the neonatally castrated subjects received a daily injection of 100 μl sesame oil (n = 8) or 1 mg TP in 100 μl oil (n = 13). Subjects were raised to adulthood, received a sham surgery, or were castrated as detailed above, and then implanted with a SILASTIC brand capsule filled with T and killed 3 wk later. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry.

    Results.

    Similar to experiment 1, neonatal castration significantly reduced AVP-ir in the LS (F(2,26) = 16.33, P 0.0001). However, treating neonatally castrated males with TP during the first week of life after neonatal castration did not maintain high levels of AVP-ir but, instead, nonsignificantly decreased AVP-ir (Fig. 5). A similar effect was found in the LHb, but the difference between the sham control males and neonatally castrated males that had received oil only approached statistical significance (P = 0.053). Neonatally castrated males that received oil or TP did not have a general deficiency in synthesizing AVP because they did not differ from each other or sham control males in the level of AVP-ir in the PVT (F(2,26) = 0.19, P 0.82; data not shown).

    FIG. 5. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS and LHb of male prairie voles that received a sham surgery at birth and then treated with oil, males that were castrated at birth and then treated with oil, or males that were castrated at birth and received TP for 7 d. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups with each brain region; P 0.05 (P 0.053 for LHb).

    Experiment 4: effects of prenatal and/or postnatal TP treatment on AVP-ir in females

    Methods.

    Pregnant female prairie voles received either a daily injection of 50 μg TP dissolved in 100 μl sesame oil (n = 16 dams) or 100 μl sesame oil (n = 16 dams) beginning 1 wk after pairing with a male until the birth of pups. As noted above, this procedure encompassed the last 2 wk of the 3-wk gestation period, as shown in our previous studies (25, 26). Beginning on the day of birth and continuing for the next 6 d, half of the litter from each prenatal treatment group was injected sc each day with 1 mg TP in 50 μl sesame oil. The other half was injected each day with 50 μl sesame oil. When they were adults, females were ovariectomized, received a T-filled capsule, and killed 3 wk later. The final sample sizes were as follows: prenatal oil/postnatal oil, n = 9; prenatal oil/postnatal TP, n = 7; prenatal TP/postnatal oil, n = 8; and prenatal TP/postnatal TP, n = 11. A group of males (n = 9) exposed to sesame oil pre- and postnatally were gonadectomized as adults, implanted with a SILASTIC brand capsule filled with T, and killed 3 wk later were the positive control.

    Results.

    All groups of females, regardless of perinatal treatment, had significantly less AVP-ir in the LS (F(4,39) = 14.46, P 0.0001; Fig. 6) and LHb (F(4,39) = 12.849, P 0.0001; Fig. 6) than control males. Two-way ANOVA on data from the LS of the female groups revealed no effect of prenatal TP (F(1,1) = 0.002, P 0.96) but a significant decrease in AVP-ir due to postnatal TP (F(1,1) = 5.41, P 0.03). There was no significant prenatal by postnatal interaction effect in the LS (F(1,1) = 0.06, P 0.81). Similar to the LS, females that received postnatal TP showed a significant decrease in AVP-ir in the LHb (F(1,1) = 9.72, P 0.005), and no effects of prenatal TP (F(1,1) = 0.34, P 0.56) or prenatal by postnatal interaction (F(1,1) = 0.01, P 0.9) were found.

    FIG. 6. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS and LHb of control male prairie voles and female prairie voles treated prenatally and/or postnatally with TP or oil. *, Males significantly greater than all groups of females within each brain region, P 0.05. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups of females within each brain region; P 0.05.

    Experiment 5: effects of a single postnatal TP administration on AVP-ir in females

    Methods.

    Females were injected once on postnatal d 3 with either 500 μg TP diluted in 50 μl sesame oil (n = 8) or sesame oil (n = 8). A group of males injected on postnatal d 3 with 50 μl sesame oil (n = 6) was used as the positive control. Subjects were gonadectomized as adults, received a T-filled capsule, and killed 3 wk later. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry.

    Results.

    A single injection of 500 μg TP on postnatal d 3, which has been reported to masculinize sexual behavior in female prairie voles (24), did not masculinize female AVP-ir to the level found in control males. Levels in both female groups were significantly lower than oil-treated control males in the LS (F(2,19) = 32.83, P 0.0001) and LHb (F(2,19) = 13.024, P 0.0003; Fig. 7). However, when just the female groups were analyzed with unpaired t tests, AVP-ir was greater in TP-treated females than oil-treated females in both the LS (t14 = 2.36, P 0.04) and LHb (t14 = 2.24, P 0.05).

    FIG. 7. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS and LHb of male and female prairie voles injected with 50 μl sesame oil or females injected with 500 μg TP once on postnatal d 3. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups within each brain region; P 0.05. Note that the differences between female groups are not statistically significant using a one-way ANOVA that includes males, but differences between female groups are significant when these two groups are compared with unpaired t tests.

    Experiment 6: effects of repeated, but spaced, neonatal administration of TP on AVP-ir in females

    Methods.

    Females were injected every third day from postnatal d 2–11 (i.e. on d 2, 5, 8, and 11) with either 500 μg TP diluted in 50 μl sesame oil (n = 9) or sesame oil (n = 8). A group of males injected on postnatal d 2, 5, 8, and 11 with 50 μl sesame oil (n = 7) was used as the positive control. Subjects were gonadectomized as adults, implanted with a T-filled capsule, and killed 3 wk later. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry.

    Results.

    Extending TP treatment past the first neonatal week had no effect on AVP-ir in females, with both groups of females having significantly lower AVP-ir than oil-treated males in the LS (F(2,21) = 9.31, P 0.002) and LHb (F(2,21) = 6.54, P 0.007; Fig. 8).

    FIG. 8. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS and LHb of male and female prairie voles injected with 50 μl sesame oil or females injected with 500 μg TP on postnatal d 2, 5, 8, and 11. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups within each brain region; P 0.05.

    Experiment 7: effects of daily neonatal administration of T, DHT, 75 μg TP, or 1000 μg TP on AVP-ir in females

    Methods.

    Females were injected daily for the first week after birth with either 50 μl sesame oil (n = 7) or 100 μg unconjugated T (n = 9), 200 μg DHT (n = 11), 75 μg TP (n = 10), or 1000 μg TP (n = 7) diluted in 50 μl sesame oil. A group of males injected the first week after birth with 50 μl sesame oil (n = 10) was used as the positive control. Subjects were gonadectomized as adults, implanted with a T-filled capsule, and killed 3 wk later. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry.

    Results.

    The LHb of one oil-treated female, one oil-treated male, and one DHT-treated female were either lost or damaged and could not be analyzed. Administration of T, DHT, or either dose of TP for the first week of life had no effects on AVP-ir in the LS of females. All groups of females were statistically similar to each other and had significantly lower AVP-ir than control males, with the exception of T-treated females, which were not significantly different than any group (F(5,48) = 2.51, P 0.05). These treatments also had no effect on AVP-ir in the LHb (F(5,45) = 4.49, P 0.003; Fig. 9).

    FIG. 9. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS and LHb of oil-treated control males, oil-treated control females, or females treated with TP (75 or 1000 μg), T (100 μg), or DHT (200 μg) daily during the first week of life. Different letters above bars indicate significant differences between groups within each brain region; P 0.05. F, Female.

    Experiment 8: effects of neonatal tamoxifen (TAM) or diethylstilbestrol (DES) on AVP-ir

    Methods.

    Males were injected each day for the first week after birth with 50 μl sesame oil (n = 11) or 20 μg of the estrogen receptor antagonist TAM dissolved in 50 μl sesame oil (n = 9). Females were injected each day for the first week after birth with either 50 μl of sesame oil (n = 12) or 1 μg of the synthetic estrogen DES diluted in 50 μl sesame oil (n = 11). Subjects were gonadectomized during adulthood, implanted with T capsules, and killed 3 wk later. Brains were later processed for AVP immunocytochemistry.

    Results.

    Males treated with TAM has significantly less AVP-ir in the LS than oil-treated males (t18 = 3.47, P 0.003), and TAM also tended to reduce AVP-ir in the LHb (t18 = 2.00, P = 0.061; Fig. 10). Females treated neonatally with DES had significantly greater AVP-ir in the LS (U = 12.50, P 0.001), but DES did not significantly affect AVP-ir in the LHb (t21 = 0.50, P 0.6; Fig. 10). In addition, there was a significant polynomial regression between the levels of AVP-ir in the LS of DES-treated females and the sex ratio of their litter at birth (F(2,10) = 4.98, P 0.04), with DES being more effective in masculinizing septal AVP-ir in females originating from litters that had more males.

    FIG. 10. AVP-ir (mean ± SEM) in the LS (top) and LHb (bottom) of male prairie voles treated neonatally with oil or TAM (left) and of female prairie voles treated neonatally with oil or DES (right). *, Significant differences between groups within sex within each brain region, P 0.05.

    Body weight and anogenital distance

    As found previously (26), neonatal castration significantly reduced adult anogenital distance in males in experiments 1–3, and TP treatment increased it to control values in neonatally castrated males in experiment 3 (all P 0.01; data not shown). Treatment with TAM had no effect on male anogenital distance (t18 = 0.19, P 0.8). Adult body weight of males was not affected by any perinatal treatment in any experiment (all P 0.1; data not shown).

    Also similar to results found previously (26), prenatal and postnatal treatment with TP significantly increased anogenital distance in the subset of females examined from experiment 4, although anogenital distance of both TP-treated and oil-treated females was shorter compared with control males (F(2,19) = 100.1, P 0.0001; data not shown). In experiment 5, injection of 500 μg TP once on postnatal d 3 did not affect anogenital distance, and the anogenital distance for both groups of females was shorter than males (F(2,19) = 77.97, P 0.0001). In Experiment 6, injection of 500 μg TP on postnatal d 2, 5, 8, and 11 also did not affect anogenital distance, and the distance was shorter for both groups of females than males (F(2,19) = 56.64, P 0.0001). In experiment 7, 1 mg TP lengthened female anogenital distance compared with other groups of females, whereas 75 μg TP, 100 μg T, and 200 μg DHT did not; anogenital distance in all groups of females was shorter than males (F(5,45) = 41.92, P 0.0001). Visual inspection of females from experiment 7 at death, however, revealed that all androgen-treated females had hypertrophied clitorises. DES treatment in experiment 8 had no effect on female anogenital distance (t21 = 1.0, P 0.3). Adult body weight of females was not affected by any perinatal treatment in any experiment (all P 0.5; data not shown).

    Discussion

    Although some sex differences in the mammalian nervous system can be influenced by the social environment or sex differences in circulating gonadal hormones during adulthood (31, 32, 33, 34), the magnitude of most identified sex differences in the rodent nervous system can be reduced or almost eliminated by manipulating gonadal hormone exposure during perinatal life. Indeed, the brains of male rats that are sheltered from perinatal androgenic or estrogenic activity through pharmacological agents prenatally and/or castration neonatally are demasculinized in many ways, including neuroendocrine release (35), structural morphology (36, 37, 38, 39, 40), hormone receptor expression (41, 42, 43), and neurotransmitter content (44, 45, 46, 47). Conversely, most of these studies have demonstrated that treatment of perinatal females with androgens or estrogens significantly masculinizes these neural characteristics.

    The same is true for the extrahypothalamic AVP pathways in rats. Castration of neonatal males (before postnatal d 7) completely demasculinizes AVP mRNA expression in the BST, as well as AVP-ir fiber density in the LS (12, 14, 48), whereas a single or repeated injection of TP to neonatally castrated males within the first 3 wk of life maintains masculinization of AVP mRNA expression in the BST and partially maintains masculinization of AVP-ir fibers in the LS (12, 13, 48). In females, injection of TP relatively soon after birth completely masculinizes AVP-ir fiber density in the LS (48), and either a single or repeated injection of TP or estradiol benzoate during this time masculinizes the number of AVP mRNA-expressing cells in the BST (12, 14).

    The present results demonstrate that neonatal castration of male prairie voles produced effects similar to those in rats, that is, completely demasculinizing AVP mRNA expression in the BST and partially demasculinizing AVP-ir fiber density in the LS and LHb. The surprising finding was that, unlike rats, perinatal treatment with exogenous TP could not maintain masculine AVP expression in neonatally castrated males and did not masculinize this system in females. A similar lack of effects was found after neonatal treatment of females with T or DHT. The 1-mg dose of TP did increase the voles’ anogenital distance, as we have reported previously (26), indicating that TP is biologically active in this species. As far as we are aware, this is the first example of a demasculinization of the male rodent brain after neonatal castration without a restoration after exogenous androgen treatment, as well the first demonstration that perinatal androgens are ineffective in masculinizing a sexually dimorphic neural trait in a female rodent. Interestingly, the same effect can be found for sex differences in the parental behavior of virgin prairie voles, such that males are demasculinized by neonatal castration but the behavior of females is not masculinized by perinatal TP (26). Petersen (49) has shown in another vole species, Microtus canicaudus, that neither neonatal castration nor treatment with flutamide or ATD feminizes the later sexual behavior of males and that females treated neonatally with TP still display normal feminine sexual behavior as adults. These unusual effects of perinatal hormones on sexual differentiation are not pervasive for every system throughout the entire genus Microtus because sexual behavior in female prairie and pine voles can, in some cases, be defeminized and masculinized by doses of TP (500–1000 μg) similar to those used in the present experiment (23, 24, 50). Although some components of sexual differentiation in prairie voles clearly differ from rats, voles are certainly not the only exceptions. Numerous other species, including domestic pigs, hamsters, ferrets, and some primates, respond to perinatal manipulations in gonadal hormones differently than laboratory rats (51), demonstrating that sexual differentiation occurs differently across species.

    Considering that the TP treatment in females from experiment 4 extended from the first week after conception through the first week after birth, it is unlikely that our manipulations missed the critical period during which this system develops. Furthermore, our findings that neonatal castration demasculinized AVP expression in males and that neonatal estrogenic manipulations affected AVP in both sexes suggest that at least part of this critical period exists during early postnatal life. In rats, extrahypothalamic AVP mRNA expression is sexually dimorphic very early in development, beginning about 3 d after birth (52). It is not known when this system first becomes sexually dimorphic in prairie voles, but considering that prairie vole pups are precocial compared with rat pups and, at birth or within a few days after, have teeth and fur and are mobile (53, 54, 55), this system probably becomes sexually dimorphic early in development in voles, and it is likely that our TP treatments encompassed this period.

    It might be suggested that prolonged treatment with exogenous androgens is somehow detrimental to this system, and we did find, in some cases, that a week-long treatment with 1000 μg TP was significantly or nonsignificantly feminizing in both sexes (experiments 3, 4, and 7). Gonadal hormones can down-regulate estradiol receptors in some areas of the developing and adult rat brain (56, 57), which could conceivably produce such a feminizing effect by not allowing sufficient estradiol receptor content to act upon. Nonetheless, repeated administration of T, DHT, lower doses of TP, or the single injection of 500 μg TP still did not masculinize the AVP system in females but was not feminizing.

    In contrast to the lack of effects of exogenous TP, early postnatal manipulations of estrogenic activity with the estrogen receptor blocker TAM in males and the synthetic estrogen DES in females influenced the development of AVP expression. This indicates that, similar to rats, estrogen is a major masculinizing factor of this system in prairie voles. The variability in the effects of DES on AVP expression in the LS of females was unexpected. There is apparently an influence of the number of male siblings on this factor, and the simplest explanation is that females gestated in male-biased litters have greater in utero exposure to their brothers’ testicular hormones, which predisposes these females’ brains to be masculinized by neonatal exposure to estrogens. A similar litter sex ratio effect has been observed for the effects of neonatal DES on sexual differentiation of the preoptic area of rats (58). It may also be considered that differences in the effects of DES between females developing within male- or female-biased litters may not necessarily be due to prenatal factors but that postnatal interactions with a large number of brothers may influence females’ neural development, as well as their responses to gonadal hormones (59).

    Although it is has been suggested that the MA is one of the sources of the sexually dimorphic extrahypothalamic AVP pathway (7), we found no sex differences in AVP mRNA expression in the MA. It is actually unclear whether this sex difference exists in rats (12, 60, 61, 62). The lack of a sex difference in the vole MA may indicate that the major contributor to sex differences in the terminal areas of the sexually dimorphic AVP projection (i.e. the LS and LHb) arises from the BST. It is also interesting to note that, although neonatal castration completely demasculinized AVP mRNA expression in the BST, it did not fully demasculinize AVP-ir in the terminal areas, indicating the lack of a one-to-one correspondence between AVP mRNA expression in the BST and AVP-ir in the LS and LHb.

    At this point, we can only conjecture about what factors differ between rats and prairie voles that might prevent perinatal androgens from masculinizing the extrahypothalamic AVP system in the latter species. We believe that there are inherent species differences in the biology underlying sexual differentiation, although one may consider that differences in their laboratory environments may play a role. For example, many laboratory rats are fed only commercially available diets high in phytoestrogens, which can cross the placenta as well as later be incorporated in maternal milk (63). Early exposure to phytoestrogens modulates reproductive development and endocrine function in rodents (64, 65) and may (66) or may not (64, 67) influence sexual differentiation of their brain. Although some of the diet provided to our laboratory voles was a commercially available rabbit chow that may contain phytoestrogens, the majority of their diet was a mixture of hay, oats, corn, and sesame seeds that may be relatively low in phytoestrogens. Therefore, perinatal hormone manipulations may influence sexual differentiation in rats and voles differently because of inadvertent differences in endocrine factors present in the diets of their mothers. Nonetheless, biological differences between these species are likely important for differences in sexual differentiation. It may be possible that the differing sex chromosomes in male and female voles directly contribute to sex differences in this system (19). It could be that testicular hormones other than testosterone may be required to be present in order for exogenous androgens to masculinize the prairie vole brain. It could also be possible that T is not the primary hormone secreted by the neonatal prairie vole testes. This might indeed be the case, and preliminary data indicate that T is nondetectable in prairie voles of either sex when killed between 18–24 h after birth, whereas estradiol levels are higher in neonatal males than in females (Lonstein, J. S., and J. A. French, preliminary data). Under some conditions, the testes of some vole species can synthesize estradiol at relatively high levels, at least in adults (68). If this is found to be the case in neonates, an intriguing extension now being followed up would be that the perinatal prairie vole brain may not need to aromatize T into its estrogenic metabolites, and relatively little aromatase enzyme may exist in their developing brain.

    Acknowledgments

    The assistance of Dr. Anna Vas and Dr. Tom Zoeller with the in situ hybridization was greatly appreciated.

    References

    Crenshaw BJ, De Vries GJ, Yahr P 1992 Vasopressin innervation of sexually dimorphic structures of the gerbil forebrain under various hormonal conditions. J Comp Neurol 322:589–598

    De Vries GJ, Buijs RM, Swaab, DF 1981 Ontogeny of the vasopressinergic neurons of the suprachiasmatic nucleus and their extrahypothalamic projections in the rat brain–presence of a sex differences in the lateral septum. Brain Res 218:67–78

    Boyd SK, Tyler CJ, De Vries GJ 1992 Sexual dimorphism in the vasotocin system of the bullfrog (Rana catesbeiana). J Comp Neurol 352:313–325

    Panzica GC, Castagana C, Viglietti-Panzica C, Russo C, Tlemcani O, Balthazart J 1998 Organizational effects of estrogens on brain vasotocin and sexual behavior in quail. J Neurobiol 37:684–699

    Foran CM, Bass AH 1999 Preoptic GnRH and AVP: axes for sexual plasticity in teleost fish. Gen Comp Endocrinol 116:141–152

    Moore RL, Richardson C, Lowry CA 2000 Sexual dimorphism in numbers of vasotocin-immunoreactive neurons in brain areas associated with reproductive behaviors in the roughskin newt. Gen Comp Endocrinol 117:281–298

    Miller M, De Vries GJ 1998 Anatomy and function of extrahypothalamic vasopressin systems in the brain. Prog Brain Res 119:3–20

    De Vries GJ 1990 Sex differences in neurotransmitter systems. J Neuroendocrinol 2:1–13

    Yahr P 1988 Sexual differentiation of behavior in the context of developmental psychobiology. In: Blass EM, ed. Handbook of behavioral neurobiology. Vol 9. New York: Plenum; 197–243

    Cooke BM, Hegstrom CD, Villeneuve LS, Breedlove MS 1998 Sexual differentiation of the vertebrate brain: principles and mechanisms. Front Neuroendocrinol 19:323–362

    Forger NG 2001 The development of sex differences in the nervous system. In: Blass EM, ed. Handbook of behavioral neurobiology. Vol 12. New York: Plenum; 153–208

    Wang Z, Bullock NA, De Vries GJ 1993 Sexual differentiation of vasopressin projection of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and medial amygdaloid nucleus in rats. Endocrinology 132:2299–2306

    Wang Z 1994 Testosterone effects on development of vasopressin messenger RNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and medial amygdaloid nucleus in male rats. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 79:147–150

    Han T, De Vries GJ 2003 Organizational effects of testosterone, estradiol, and dihydrotestosterone on vasopressin mRNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis. J Neurobiol 54:502–510

    Buijs RM, Pevet P, Masson-Pevet M, Pool CW, De Vries GJ, Canguilhem B, Vivien-Roels B 1986 Seasonal variation in vasopressin innervation in the brain of the European hamster (Cricetus cricetus). Brain Res 371:193–196

    Hermes ML, Buijs RM, Masson-Pevet M, Pevet P 1990 Seasonal changes in vasopressin in the brain of the garden dormouse (Eliomys quercinus L.). J Comp Neurol 293:340–346

    Bamshad M, Novak MA, De Vries GJ 1993 Sex and species differences in the vasopressin innervation of sexually naive and parental prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster and meadow voles, Microtus pennsylvanicus. J Neuroendocrinol 5:247–255

    Wang Z, Smith W, Major DE, De Vries GJ 1994 Sex and species differences in the effects of cohabitation on vasopressin messenger RNA expression in the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) and meadow voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Brain Res 650:212–218

    De Vries GJ, Rissman EF, Simerly RB, Yang LY, Scordalakes EM, Auger CJ, Swain A, Lovell-Badge R, Burgoyne PS, Arnold AP 2002 A model system for study of sex chromosome effects on sexual dimorphic neural and behavioral traits. J Neurosci 22:9005–9014

    Baum MJ 1979 Differentiation of coital behavior in mammals: a comparative analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 3:265–284

    Carter CS, De Vries AC, Getz LL 1995 Physiological substrates of mammalian monogamy: the prairie vole model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 19:303–314

    Pfaff DW, Zigmond RE 1971 Neonatal androgen effects on sexual and non-sexual behavior of adult rats tested under various hormone regimes. Neuroendocrinology 7:129–145

    Roberts RL, Zullo AS, Carter CS 1997 Sexual differentiation in prairie voles: the effects of corticosterone and testosterone. Physiol Behav 62:1379–1383

    Smale L, Nelson RJ, Zucker I 1985 Neuroendocrine responsiveness to oestradiol and male urine in neonatally androgenized prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). J Reprod Fertil 74:491–496

    Lonstein JS, De Vries GJ 2000 Influence of gonadal hormones in the development of parental behavior in adult virgin prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav Brain Res 114:79–87

    Lonstein JS, Rood BD, De Vries GJ 2002 Parental responsiveness is feminized after neonatal castration in virgin male prairie voles, but is not masculinized by perinatal testosterone in virgin females. Horm Behav 41:80–87

    Wang Z, De Vries GJ 1993 Testosterone effects on paternal behavior and vasopressin immunoreactive projections in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Brain Res 631:156–160

    Lonstein JS, De Vries GJ 2000 Sex differences in the parental behavior of rodents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 24:669–686

    Lonstein JS, De Vries GJ 1999 Sex differences in the parental behaviour of adult virgin prairie voles: independence from gonadal hormones and vasopressin. J Neuroendocrinol 11:441–449

    Paxinos G, Watson 1998 The rat brain in stereotaxic coordinates. 4th ed. New York: Academic Press

    Simerly RB 1990 Hormonal control of neuropeptide gene expression in sexually dimorphic olfactory pathways. Trends Neurosci 13:104–110

    Breedlove SM, Cooke BM, Jordan CL 1999 The orthodox view of brain sexual differentiation. Brain Behav Evol 54:8–14

    Cooke BM, Chowanadisai W, Breedlove SM 2000 Post-weaning social isolation of male rats reduces the volume of the medial amygdala and leads to deficits in adult sexual behavior. Behav Brain Res 117:107–113

    Cooke BM, Tabibnia G, Breedlove SM 1999 A brain sexual dimorphism controlled by adult circulating androgens. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 96:7538–7540

    Becu-Villalobos D, Gonzalez Iglesias A, Diaz-Torga G, Hockl P, Libertun C 1997 Brain sexual differentiation and gonadotropins secretion in the rat. Cell Mol Neurobiol 17:699–715

    Jacobson CD, Csernus VJ, Shryne JE, Gorski RA 1981 The influence of gonadectomy, androgen exposure, or a gonadal graft in the neonatal rat on the volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area. J Neurosci 1:1142–1147

    Del Abril A, Segovia S, Guillamon A 1987 The bed nucleus of the stria terminalis in the rat: regional sex differences controlled by gonadal steroids early after birth. Brain Res 429:295–300

    Fitch RH, Cowell PE, Schrott LM, Denenberg VH 1991 Corpus callosum: demasculinization via perinatal anti-androgen. Int J Dev Neurosci 9:35–38

    Nunez JL, Jurgens HA, Juraska JM 2000 Androgens reduce cell death in the developing rat visual cortex. Dev Brain Res 125:83–88

    Vinader-Caerols C, Callado P, Segovia S, Guillamon A 2000 Estradiol masculinizes the posteromedial cortical nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. Brain Res Bull 53:269–273

    Bakker J, Pool CW, Sonnemans M, Van Leeuwen FW, Slob AK 1997 Quantitative estimation of estrogen and androgen receptor-immunoreactive cells in the forebrain of neonatally estrogen-deprived male rats. Neuroscience 77:911–919

    McAbee MD, DonCarlos LL 1999 Estrogen, but not androgens, regulates androgen receptor messenger ribonucleic acid expression in the developing male rat forebrain. Endocrinology 140:3674–3681

    Quadros PS, Goldstein AY, De Vries GJ, Wagner CK 2002 Regulation of sex differences in progesterone receptor expression in the medial preoptic nucleus of postnatal rats. J Neuroendocrinol 14:761–767

    Simerly RB, Swanson LW, Gorski RA 1985 Reversal of the sexually dimorphic distribution of serotonin-immunoreactive fibers in the medial preoptic nucleus by treatment with perinatal androgen. Brain Res 340:91–98

    Simerly RB 1989 Hormonal control of the development and regulation of tyrosine hydroxylase expression within a sexually dimorphic population of dopaminergic cells in the hypothalamus. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 6:297–310

    Siddiqui A, Shah BH 1997 Neonatal androgen manipulation differentially affects the development of monoamine systems in rat cerebral cortex, amygdala and hypothalamus. Dev Brain Res 98:247–252

    Davis AM, Ward SC, Selmanoff M, Herbison AE, McCarthy MM 1999 Developmental sex differences in amino acid neurotransmitter levels in hypothalamic and limbic areas of rat brain. Neuroscience 90:1471–1482

    De Vries GJ, Best W, Sluiter AA 1983 The influence of androgens on the development of a sex differences in the vasopressinergic innervation of the rat lateral septum. Brain Res 284:377–380

    Petersen SL 1986 Perinatal androgen manipulations do not affect feminine behavioral potentials in voles. Physiol Behav 36:527–531

    Wekesa KS, Vandenbergh JG 1996 Androgen exposure and reproductive behavior in an induced ovulator, the pine vole (Microtus pinetorum). Horm Behav 30:416–423

    Wallin K, Baum MJ 2002 Masculinization and defeminization in altricial and precocial mammals: comparative aspects of steroid hormone action. In: Pfaff DW, Arnold AP, Etgen AM, Fahrbach SE, Rubin RT, eds. Hormones, brain, and behavior. Vol 4. New York: Academic Press; 385–424

    Szot P, Dorsa DM 1993 Differential timing and sexual dimorphism in the expression of vasopressin gene in the developing rat brain. Dev Brain Res 73:177–183

    Krukenberg SM, Gier HT, Dennis SM 1973 Postnatal development of the prairie vole, Microtus ochrogaster. Lab Anim Sci 23:53–55

    Salo AL, Shapiro LE, Dewsbury DA 1994 Comparisons of nipple attachment and incisor growth among four species of voles (Microtus). Dev Psychobiol 27:317–330

    Wang Z, Novak MA 1994 Alloparental care and the influence of father presence on juvenile prairie voles, Microtus ochrogaster. Anim Behav 47:281–288

    DonCarlos LL, McAbee M, Ramer-Quinn DS, Stancik DM 1995 Estrogen receptor mRNA levels in the preoptic area of neonatal rats are responsive to hormone manipulation. Dev Brain Res 84:253–260

    Lisciotto CA, Morrell JI 1993 Circulating gonadal steroid hormones regulate estrogen receptor mRNA in the male rat forebrain. Brain Res Mol Brain Res 20:79–90

    Faber KA, Ayyash L, Dixon S, Hughes CL 1993 Effect of neonatal diethylstilbestrol exposure on volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area and pituitary responsiveness to gonadotropin-releasing hormone in female rats of known anogenital distance at birth. Biol Reprod 48:947–951

    Morgan LR, Hite RF, Cushing BS 1997 Exposure to male siblings facilitates the response to estradiol in sexually naive female prairie voles. Physiol Behav 61:955–956

    Wang Z, De Vries GJ 1995 Androgen and estrogen effects on vasopressin messenger RNA expression in the medial amygdaloid nucleus in male and female rats. J Neuroendocrinology 7:827–831

    Al-Shamma HA, De Vries GJ 1996 Neurogenesis of the sexually dimorphic vasopressin cells of the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis and amygdala of rats. J Neurobiol 29:91–98

    De Vries GJ, Al-Shamma HA 1990 Sex differences in hormonal responses of vasopressin pathways in the rat brain. J Neurobiol 21:686–693

    Brown NM, Setchell KD 2001 Animal models impacted by phytoestrogens in commercial chow: implications for pathways influenced by hormones. Lab Invest 81:735–747

    Masutomi N, Shibutani M, Takagi H, Uneyama C, Takahashi N, Hirose M 2003 Impact of dietary exposure to methoxychlor, genistein, or diisononyl phthalate during the perinatal period on the development of the rat endocrine/reproductive systems in later life. Toxicology 192:149–170

    Thigpen JE, Haseman JK, Saunders HE, Setchell KD, Grant MG, Forsythe DB 2003 Dietary phytoestrogens accelerate the time of vaginal opening in immature CD-1 mice. Comp Med 53:607–615

    Faber KA, Hughes CL 1993 Dose-response characteristics of neonatal exposure to genistein on pituitary responsiveness to gonadotropin releasing hormone and volume of the sexually dimorphic nucleus of the preoptic area (SDN-POA) in postpubertal castrated female rats. Reprod Toxicol 7:35–39

    Weber KS, Setchell KD, Lephart ED 2001 Maternal and perinatal brain aromatase: effects of dietary soy phytoestrogens. Brain Res Dev Brain Res 126:217–221

    Bilinska B, Schmalz-Fraczek B, Kotula M, Carreau S 2001 Photoperiod-dependent capability of androgen aromatization and the role of estrogen in the bank vole testis visualized by means of immunohistochemistry. Mol Cell Endocrinol 178:189–198(Joseph S. Lonstein, Benja)