当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《新英格兰医药杂志》 > 2006年第26期 > 正文
编号:11327543
A Controlled Trial of Homocysteine Lowering and Cognitive Performance
http://www.100md.com 《新英格兰医药杂志》
     ABSTRACT

    Background The results of observational studies suggest that plasma homocysteine concentrations are inversely related to cognitive function in older people. Our objective was to test the hypothesis that lowering the plasma homocysteine concentration improves cognitive function in healthy older people.

    Methods We conducted a two-year, double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial involving 276 healthy participants, 65 years of age or older, with plasma homocysteine concentrations of at least 13 μmol per liter. Homocysteine-lowering treatment was a daily supplement containing folate (1000 μg) and vitamins B12 (500 μg) and B6 (10 mg). Tests of cognition were conducted at baseline and after one and two years of treatment. Treatment effects were adjusted for baseline values, sex, and education.

    Results On average, during the course of the study, the plasma homocysteine concentration was 4.36 μmol per liter (95 percent confidence interval, 3.81 to 4.91 μmol per liter) lower in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (P<0.001). Overall, there were no significant differences between the vitamin and placebo groups in the scores on tests of cognition.

    Conclusions The results of this trial do not support the hypothesis that homocysteine lowering with B vitamins improves cognitive performance. (Australian Clinical Trials registry number, ACTR NO 12605000030673.)

    The prevalence of cognitive impairment increases with age and represents a major public health concern in aging populations. There is evidence that circulating homocysteine concentrations may be a modifiable risk factor for cognitive decline.1 Homocysteine concentrations have been reported to be higher in persons with suspected or confirmed Alzheimer's disease than in age-matched controls.2,3,4 Alzheimer's disease was more likely to develop over an eight-year period in persons with plasma homocysteine concentrations above 14 μmol per liter than among those with lower concentrations.5 Furthermore, the results of several cross-sectional and prospective studies of community-based older adults indicate that homocysteine is inversely associated with performance on some tests of cognition.6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15

    The inverse relation between homocysteine concentrations and blood concentrations of folate and vitamin B12 makes it difficult to disentangle the independent effects of each on cognitive function.16 Severe deficiency of folate or vitamin B12 leads to cognitive impairment.17,18 At concentrations of these vitamins not normally associated with deficiency, associations with lower performance on various tests of cognition have been reported in several observational studies involving healthy older people.10,12,13,14,15,19,20

    Supplements containing folate, with or without vitamins B12 and B6, decrease homocysteine concentrations.21 In two randomized, controlled trials of less than four months' duration, lowering homocysteine with B vitamins had no effect on scores of cognition in people at high risk for dementia22 or in community-dwelling older people.23 Longer-term trials are required to determine whether lowering homocysteine concentrations influences cognitive function in healthy older people. We conducted a two-year clinical trial to test the hypothesis that in older people with elevated plasma homocysteine concentrations, decreasing homocysteine with B vitamins would result in better cognitive performance.

    Methods

    Participants

    This double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial of two years' duration was conducted in Dunedin, New Zealand, between August 2002 and December 2004. The University of Otago Human Ethics Committee approved the trial, and written informed consent was obtained from all participants before screening. Volunteers 65 years of age or older were recruited from service clubs (e.g., Rotary International), through advertisements in newspapers, and by direct mail. Participants were ineligible if they had suspected dementia; were taking medications known to interfere with folate metabolism (e.g., oral hypoglycemic agents or antiepileptic agents); were taking vitamin supplements containing folic acid, vitamin B12, or vitamin B6; were being treated for depression; had diabetes; or had a history of stroke or transient ischemic attacks.

    Eligible participants attended a screening clinic at which a blood sample was collected after an overnight fast for measurement of plasma homocysteine and creatinine. Tests of cognition were not administered at the screening visit. Those with a fasting homocysteine concentration of at least 13 μmol per liter and a normal plasma creatinine concentration (133 μmol per liter in men and 115 μmol per liter in women) were invited to participate in the trial.

    Before randomization, all eligible participants were stratified according to the median values for age and homocysteine concentration in the screening population. Random decimals between 0 and 1 were generated for each person in each of the four strata. Those below the median of the random numbers in each stratum were assigned to the vitamin group, and the remainder were assigned to the placebo group. Participants were asked to consume one capsule daily for two years. The placebo capsules contained a blend of magnesium stearate and microcrystalline cellulose as a filler, whereas the treatment capsules contained the filler plus 1000 μg of folate (the calcium salt of L-5-methyltetrahydrofolate), 500 μg of vitamin B12 (cobalamin), and 10 mg of vitamin B6 (pyridoxine) (Merck Eprova). All capsules were gelatin-coated, identical in color and shape, and packaged in blister packs. Compliance was assessed by counting returned capsules. The primary end points were the scores on tests of cognition, and the secondary end points were the biochemical measurements.

    End Points

    Neuropsychological tests were administered at baseline and at one and two years to assess a broad range of the participants' cognitive functions. To minimize the effect of learning and practice, alternate forms of the tests (e.g., various paragraphs, letters, word categories, or matrices) were used where possible.

    The Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE) was included to provide a global measure of cognitive function and to assess participants for dementia.24,25 Memory and learning capacity were assessed with the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test24,26 and paragraph-recall tests from the Wechsler Memory Scales.24,27 The 15-item parallel-word lists used for the administration of the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test are described by Lezak,24 and the total score on the first five trials (trials I through V) and the score on the delayed trial (trial VII) were used as indicators of learning and recall ability, respectively. Verbal fluency was assessed with use of the Controlled Oral Word Association Test of the Multilingual Aphasia Examination,28 which requires participants to generate as many words as possible with a specified initial letter in 60 seconds. Three letters were used on each occasion: C, F, and L at baseline; P, R, and W at one year; and F, A, and S at two years. Semantic fluency was assessed with use of the Category Word Fluency test as described by Lezak,24 with the score being the total number of words generated in a specific category in 60 seconds. On each occasion, participants were instructed to generate words from three specified categories (e.g., animals, fruit and vegetables, and means of transportation) in 60 seconds.

    As a measure of information-processing speed, the Reitan Trail Making Test was administered, and the time taken to complete Part B, which measures visual search and attentional processes, was used in the subsequent analysis. At baseline and the two-year follow-up, the original version of the Trail Making Test was administered; at the one-year follow-up, the order of the letter and digit stimulus elements was reversed.24,26,29 Finally, as a measure of reasoning ability, a different set of 20 items from the Raven's Progressive Matrices was administered on each occasion. At baseline, the set comprised item 1 and every third item thereafter; the other two sets were constructed in the same way, starting with item 2 and item 3. The National Adult Reading Test,24,30 which assesses the ability to read aloud words of irregular pronunciation, was used to estimate the IQ at baseline. One of the authors administered all the cognitive tests. The tests were conducted in the same order during each session, and whenever possible, the one-year and two-year tests for a given participant were carried out at the same time on the same day of the week as the baseline tests.

    Secondary end points were plasma concentrations of homocysteine, folate, and vitamin B12. Blood samples were collected at baseline and every six months by venipuncture after a 10-to-12-hour overnight fast. Plasma was obtained by centrifuging whole blood at 1650xg for 15 minutes at 4°C within 2 hours after collection. Blood samples were stored at –80°C until analyzed. Total L-homocysteine in plasma was measured with the IMx fluorescence polarization immunoassay (Abbott). The five samples (obtained at baseline and at 6, 12, 18, and 24 months) from each subject were analyzed in the same batch to reduce variation. Plasma folate concentrations were determined by a microtiter technique, exactly as described by O'Broin and Kelleher,31 with chloramphenicol-resistant Lactobacillus casei as the test microorganism. Plasma vitamin B12 was measured with the Advia Centaur vitamin B12 assay, a competitive immunoassay based on direct chemiluminescent technology. Plasma creatinine was measured colorimetrically with kits on a Cobas Mira analyzer (Roche Diagnostics). Coefficients of variation for these assays were 6.7 percent for plasma homocysteine, 7.7 percent for plasma folate, 5.6 percent for plasma vitamin B12, and 7.2 percent for plasma creatinine.

    Statistical Analysis

    We estimated that 100 participants in each of the two study groups would be required for us to detect a minimum treatment effect size of 0.4 for the Controlled Oral Word Association Test (standard deviation, 10.5) and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test, trials I through V (standard deviation, 7.5), with a power of 80 percent and a two-sided alpha level of 0.05.32 Allowing for a dropout rate of 10 percent and death rate of 13 percent, we recruited 130 participants per group. Differences in the characteristics of the participants in the vitamin and placebo groups at baseline were determined with use of Student's t-test for continuous variables and the chi-square test or Fisher's exact test for categorical variables. Generalized estimating equations with an exchangeable correlation matrix and robust standard errors were used to analyze the data (Stata 8.0).

    The results are presented as estimates (with 95 percent confidence intervals) of the difference between the two treatments, for both time periods combined, for each test of cognition. The difference between the treatments for each variable was estimated after adjusting for its baseline values in the first model and its baseline values, sex, and education in the second model. Because of their positive skew, scores for Part B of the Reitan Trail Making Test were log-transformed before analysis, and the results were presented as a ratio. To make the results easier to compare, an effect size for each test was calculated by dividing the difference between the treatments by the standard deviation of the test result for the whole sample at baseline. A generalized estimating equation was used to estimate a combined effect for the difference between treatments. The scores for each test were converted to standard deviation scores by dividing each score by the standard deviation at baseline; the result was then analyzed in a model that included a term for each test and that adjusted for its baseline value, sex, and education. Interim analyses were not conducted during the course of the study. The primary end points, including the combined score, and statistical comparisons were prespecified. All P values were two-sided and were not adjusted for multiple testing.

    The authors designed the study, gathered and analyzed the data, and wrote the manuscript. Dr. Skeaff is the guarantor of the data and the analysis.

    Results

    Participants

    Of the 465 people who were screened, 175 were excluded because they had a fasting plasma homocysteine level of less than 13 μmol per liter (172 people) or an abnormal plasma creatinine level (3 people) (Figure 1). An additional 14 people declined to participate in the intervention after screening. The remaining 276 people were randomly assigned — 138 to the vitamin group and 138 to the placebo group. Three participants withdrew before baseline values were collected. Twelve participants in the placebo group and 11 in the vitamin group were lost to follow-up. Fifteen participants discontinued taking the supplements but completed the tests of cognition and were included in the final analysis. The statistical analysis of the end points included 253 participants. The characteristics of the two groups at baseline are shown in Table 1. Five participants (three in the placebo group and two in the vitamin group) had a baseline MMSE score of less than 26. Overall, 215 of the participants (85 percent) took at least 95 percent of their study capsules. Equal numbers in both groups (2.6 percent) reported side effects associated with taking the capsules; effects included swallowing difficulties and increased thirst.

    Figure 1. Study Participation and Follow-up.

    Table 1. Baseline Characteristics of the Participants.

    End Points

    The plasma total homocysteine concentration was lower and the folate and vitamin B12 concentrations were higher in the vitamin group than in the placebo group at every time point (Figure 2). At six months, the homocysteine concentration was 4.23 μmol per liter (95 percent confidence interval, 3.56 to 4.90 μmol per liter) lower in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (P<0.001). The mean difference between the two groups in the plasma homocysteine concentration, averaged for months 6, 12, 18, and 24, was 4.36 μmol per liter (95 percent confidence interval, 3.81 to 4.91 μmol per liter) (P<0.001). Plasma folate and vitamin B12 concentrations were higher by 22.8 ng per milliliter (51.7 nmol per liter) (95 percent confidence interval, 21.1 to 24.5 ng per milliliter ) and 355 pg per milliliter (262 pmol per liter) (95 percent confidence interval, 309 to 401 pg per milliliter ), respectively, in the vitamin group at six months (P<0.001 for both comparisons).

    Figure 2. Effect of Vitamins on Biochemical Measurements.

    I bars represent 95 percent confidence intervals. Asterisks indicate a significant difference from the placebo group (P<0.001). To convert the values for folate to nanograms per milliliter, divide by 2.266. To convert the values for vitamin B12 to picograms per milliliter, divide by 0.738.

    The mean score on the Wechsler Paragraph Recall test was lower in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (difference, –1.19; 95 percent confidence interval, –2.30 to –0.04; P=0.03), but the difference did not remain significant after adjustment for sex and education (–0.88; 95 percent confidence interval, –1.98 to 0.21; P=0.12) (Table 2). On Part B of the Reitan Trail Making Test, the mean time to completion of the test was 7 percent longer in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (95 percent confidence interval, 2 to 13 percent; P=0.009). Further adjustment for sex and education did not alter the difference (8 percent; 95 percent confidence interval, 2 to 14 percent; P=0.007). The differences in cognitive scores between the vitamin and placebo groups are shown in Figure 3 as standard deviation scores. Part B of the Reitan Trail Making Test was the only individual test with an effect size different from zero, at –0.20 (95 percent confidence interval, –0.34 to –0.05; P=0.007). The combined treatment score for the eight tests of cognition was –0.11 standard deviation scores poorer in the vitamin group than in the placebo group (95 percent confidence interval, –0.22 to 0; P=0.05).

    Table 2. Cognition-Test Scores and Differences in Scores between the Two Groups during the Study Period.

    Figure 3. Effect of Vitamins on Cognitive Function.

    For the combined score, a generalized estimating equation was used to estimate a combined effect for the difference between treatments. The scores for each test were converted to standard deviation scores by dividing the score by the standard deviation at baseline; the result was then analyzed in a model that included a term for each test and that adjusted for its baseline value, sex, and education. Interim analyses were not conducted during the course of the study. Horizontal bars represent 95 percent confidence intervals. The result of Part B of the Trail Making Test was worse with vitamins than with placebo (P=0.007). The combined score was worse with vitamins than with placebo (P=0.05).

    Discussion

    In this two-year, double-blind, placebo-controlled, randomized clinical trial, daily use of a supplement containing folate and vitamins B12 and B6 reduced plasma homocysteine concentrations in healthy older people by 4.36 μmol per liter. There were no significant differences between scores on the tests of cognition in the vitamin and placebo groups, with the exception of Part B of the Reitan Trail Making Test, with participants in the vitamin group taking longer than those in the placebo group to complete the test. We chose a range of tests to assess cognition; thus, it is conceivable that, with the large number of statistical comparisons, the results relating to this test are a chance finding.

    Many observational studies suggest an inverse association between homocysteine concentrations and cognitive performance.6,7,8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15 Morris et al. reported an inverse association between homocysteine concentration and cognition on two tests of short delayed recall in 1200 healthy older Americans.8 In a cross-sectional analysis of 2096 dementia- and stroke-free participants in the Framingham Offspring Study, plasma homocysteine was inversely associated with performance on a range of cognitive tests in persons 60 years of age or older.33

    Several elements of our study lend weight to the veracity of the findings. The trial had sufficient power to detect a clinically meaningful difference in cognitive function between the treatments, few participants were lost to follow-up, compliance with supplement use was high, and there was a marked increase in plasma concentrations of folate and B12 in the vitamin group, causing a large decrease in homocysteine concentrations. Furthermore, the participants were selected on the basis of an elevated homocysteine concentration and did not have dementia or a history of stroke. Finally, we used a variety of tests to assess a range of cognitive abilities.

    On the other hand, several factors may limit the extent to which the results may be generalized. The trial may not have been long enough to establish the true effect of decreasing plasma homocysteine on cognitive performance, and thus we cannot rule out the possibility of a benefit with long-term treatment; in addition, the trial did not address the issue of whether homocysteine lowering reduces the risk of dementia. Moreover, we cannot rule out the possibility that lower doses of vitamins would have led to a different result. The participants were a healthy group of older people with high MMSE scores and a high level of education but were selected on the basis of a plasma homocysteine concentration of at least 13 μmol per liter. It is possible that a group with dementia or mild cognitive impairment, lower MMSE scores, a different level of education, or a higher initial homocysteine concentration might have had a different response.

    Neuropsychological tests are highly intercorrelated, so a composite score will be a valid and reliable measure of global cognitive capacity. Nonetheless, since each test measures a specific aspect of cognition that may be differentially responsive to treatment, the aggregation of test scores may obscure any such effect. For this reason, we analyzed the test scores both singly and in combination. The combined score of the cognitive tests was lower in the vitamin group than in the placebo group. This finding is consistent with a report by Morris et al., who observed an association between high folate intake and accelerated cognitive decline over a six-year period among 3718 older people living in Chicago.34 However, the magnitude of the difference in the combined score between the vitamin and placebo groups was small, with an effect size of –0.11 (P=0.05). Furthermore, the effect was in the opposite direction to that suggested by the weight of a substantial body of observational evidence. In summary, the results of our trial do not support the hypothesis that homocysteine lowering with folate, vitamins B12, and B6 improves cognitive performance in healthy older people.

    Supported by an Otago Research Grant.

    No potential conflict of interest relevant to this article was reported.

    We are indebted to Merck Eprova for providing the vitamin and placebo capsules.

    Source Information

    From the Departments of Human Nutrition (J.A.M., T.J.G., C.M.S., J.I.M.), Psychology (R.G.K.), and Preventive and Social Medicine (S.M.W.), University of Otago, Dunedin, New Zealand.

    Address reprint requests to Dr. Skeaff at the Department of Human Nutrition, University of Otago, P.O. Box 56, Dunedin, New Zealand, or at murray.skeaff@stonebow.otago.ac.nz.

    References

    Selhub J, Bagley LC, Miller J, Rosenberg IH. B vitamins, homocysteine, and neurocognitive function in the elderly. Am J Clin Nutr 2000;71:614S-620S.

    Joosten E, Lesaffre E, Riezler R, et al. Is metabolic evidence for vitamin B-12 and folate deficiency more frequent in elderly patients with Alzheimer's disease? J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 1997;52:M76-M79.

    Clarke R, Smith AD, Jobst KA, Refsum H, Sutton L, Ueland PM. Folate, vitamin B12, and serum total homocysteine levels in confirmed Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol 1998;55:1449-1455.

    McCaddon A, Davies G, Hudson P, Tandy S, Cattell H. Total serum homocysteine in senile dementia of Alzheimer type. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry 1998;13:235-239.

    Seshadri SA, Beiser A, Selhub J, et al. Plasma homocysteine as a risk factor for dementia and Alzheimer's disease. N Engl J Med 2002;346:476-483.

    Riggs KM, Spiro A III, Tucker K, Rush D. Relations of vitamin B-12, vitamin B-6, folate, and homocysteine to cognitive performance in the Normative Aging Study. Am J Clin Nutr 1996;63:306-314.

    McCaddon A, Hudson P, Davies G, Hughes A, Williams JH, Wilkinson C. Homocysteine and cognitive decline in healthy elderly. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord 2001;12:309-313.

    Morris MS, Jacques PF, Rosenberg IH, Selhub J. Hyperhomocysteinemia associated with poor recall in the Third National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. Am J Clin Nutr 2001;73:927-933.

    Ravaglia G, Forti P, Maioli F, et al. Homocysteine and cognitive function in healthy elderly community dwellers in Italy. Am J Clin Nutr 2003;77:668-673.

    Duthie SJ, Whalley LJ, Collins AR, Leaper S, Berger K, Deary IJ. Homocysteine, B vitamin status, and cognitive function in the elderly. Am J Clin Nutr 2002;75:908-913.

    Budge M, Johnston C, Hogervorst E, et al. Plasma total homocysteine and cognitive performance in a volunteer elderly population. Ann N Y Acad Sci 2000;903:407-410.

    Tucker KL, Qiao N, Scott T, Rosenberg I, Spiro A III. High homocysteine and low B vitamins predict cognitive decline in aging men: the Veterans Affairs Normative Aging Study. Am J Clin Nutr 2005;82:627-635.

    Kado DM, Karlamangla AS, Huang MH, et al. Homocysteine versus the vitamins folate, B6, and B12 as predictors of cognitive function and decline in older high-functioning adults: MacArthur Studies of Successful Aging. Am J Med 2005;118:161-167.

    Mooijaart SP, Gussekloo J, Frolich M, et al. Homocysteine, vitamin B-12, and folic acid and the risk of cognitive decline in old age: the Leiden 85-Plus Study. Am J Clin Nutr 2005;82:866-871.

    Nurk E, Refsum H, Tell GS, et al. Plasma total homocysteine and memory in the elderly: the Hordaland Homocysteine Study. Ann Neurol 2005;58:847-857.

    Selhub J, Jacques PF, Wilson PW, Rush D, Rosenberg IH. Vitamin status and intake as primary determinants of homocysteinemia in an elderly population. JAMA 1993;270:2693-2698.

    Herbert V. Experimental nutritional folate deficiency in man. Trans Assoc Am Physicians 1962;75:307-320.

    Lindenbaum J, Healton EB, Savage DG, et al. Neuropsychiatric disorders caused by cobalamin deficiency in the absence of anemia or macrocytosis. N Engl J Med 1988;318:1720-1728.

    Goodwin JS, Goodwin JM, Garry PJ. Association between nutritional status and cognitive functioning in a healthy elderly population. JAMA 1983;249:2917-2921.

    Lindeman RD, Romero LJ, Koehler KM, et al. Serum vitamin B12, C and folate concentrations in the New Mexico elder health survey: correlations with cognitive and affective functions. J Am Coll Nutr 2000;19:68-76.

    Homocysteine Lowering Trialists' Collaboration. Lowering blood homocysteine with folic acid based supplements: meta-analysis of randomised trials. BMJ 1998;316:894-898.

    Clarke R, Harrison G, Richards S. Effect of vitamins and aspirin on markers of platelet activation, oxidative stress and homocysteine in people at high risk of dementia. J Intern Med 2003;254:67-75.

    Lewerin C, Matousek M, Steen G, Johansson B, Steen B, Nilsson-Ehle H. Significant correlations of plasma homocysteine and serum methylmalonic acid with movement and cognitive performance in elderly subjects but no improvement from short-term vitamin therapy: a placebo-controlled randomized study. Am J Clin Nutr 2005;81:1155-1162.

    Lezak MD. Neuropsychological assessment. 3rd ed. New York: Oxford University Press, 1995.

    Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-Mental State": a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res 1975;12:189-198.

    Crawford JR, Stewart LE, Moore JW. Demonstration of savings on the AVLT and development of a parallel form. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol 1989;11:975-981.

    Wechsler D. Wechsler Memory Scale:manual. 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex.: Psychological Corporation, 1997.

    Benton A, Hamsher K. Multilingual Aphasia Examination. Iowa City, Iowa: AJA, 1989.

    Reitan RM. The relation of the Trail Making Test to organic brain damage. J Consult Psychol 1955;19:393-394.

    Nelson HE, Willisson J. National Adult Reading Test (NART): test manual. 2nd ed. Berkshire, England: NFER-Nelson Publishing, 1991.

    O'Broin S, Kelleher B. Microbiological assay on microtitre plates of folate in serum and red cells. J Clin Pathol 1992;45:344-347.

    Mitrushina MM, Boone KB, D'Elia LF. Handbook of normative data for neuropsychological assessment. New York: Oxford University Press, 1999.

    Elias MF, Sullivan LM, D'Agostino RB, et al. Homocysteine and cognitive performance in the Framingham Offspring Study: age is important. Am J Epidemiol 2005;162:644-653.

    Morris MS, Jacques PF, Selhub J. Relation between homocysteine and B-vitamin status indicators and bone mineral density in older Americans. Bone 2005;37:234-242.(Jennifer A. McMahon, Ph.D)