当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《国际神经病学神经外科学杂志》 > 2004年第2期 > 正文
编号:11355815
Walking difficulties in patients with Alzheimer’s disease might originate from gait apraxia
http://www.100md.com 《神经病学神经外科学杂志》
     1 Neuropsychology Research Group, School of Psychology, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, UK

    2 Third Neurology Ward, Department of Medicine, Surgery and Dentistry, University of Milan, S Paolo Hospital, Milan, Italy

    Correspondence to:

    Dr Sergio Della Sala

    Psychology Department, University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen AB24 2UB, UK; sergio@abdn.ac.uk

    ABSTRACT

    Objectives: To investigate whether gait apraxia is a possible cause for some of the walking abnormalities shown by patients with Alzheimer’s disease.

    Methods: 60 patients with Alzheimer’s disease, selected as being free from overt extrapyramidal impairment or other potential causes of walking deficits, were assessed with a new test evaluating aspects of walking and related movements. Norms for this test were collected from a sample of 182 healthy volunteers.

    Results: 40% of the Alzheimer group performed below the cut off score on this test, and half performed poorly. Performance of the Alzheimer group in the walking skills test correlated highly with scores in a test assessing limb apraxia and with dementia severity.

    Conclusions: Gait apraxia may be the cause of walking disorders found in a subgroup of patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Its detection is made easier by the use of a standardised test, but still relies heavily on the exclusion of other causes of walking deficits. It is a recognisable and independent form of apraxia.

    Keywords: walking deficits; gait apraxia; limb apraxia; Alzheimer’s disease

    Abbreviations: AWS, assessment of walking skills; MODA, Milan overall dementia assessment

    Some patients with Alzheimer’s disease have walking difficulties.1 When these difficulties occur, patients walk with slow and irregular steps and find it hard to negotiate turns, climb onto a stepping stool, avoid obstacles in their path, or lie down and rise from the doctor’s couch.2–4 A study showed that about three years after diagnosis, 50% of Alzheimer patients reported problems in walking, and of these 33% were classified as non-ambulatory.3 These clinical observations have been confirmed by experiments that showed that patients with Alzheimer’s disease walk more slowly than healthy elderly people.5,6 These walking problems are acknowledged in most reviews of motor deficits in Alzheimer’s disease7 and are interpreted as manifestations of the extrapyramidal deficits which affect 12–28% of Alzheimer patients, or as side effects of drug treatment—for example, with neuroleptic agents.8,9 However, Alzheimer patients without apparent extrapyramidal signs may also show overt problems in walking and trunk movements. Kurlan et al10 maintain that a proportion of these individuals may be affected by "frontal gait disorder,"11 a syndrome coterminous with gait apraxia. A similar view was championed by Visser.12

    Meyer and Barron13 defined gait apraxia as "the loss of ability to properly use the lower limbs in the act of walking." Gait apraxia11,13–16 includes disturbances of trunk movements, standing, and walking (Rumpf, Stand und Gangapraxie,17) that are not caused by orthopaedic abnormalities (for example, bone and joint degeneration), muscle wasting, arteriosclerosis obliterans of the lower limbs, pyramidal deficits, ataxia (cerebellar, vestibular or proprioceptive), dystonias and dyskinesias (from diseases that involve the basal ganglia), psychiatric disease (for example, schizophrenic mannerisms18), side effects of drugs,19 or "cautious gait" because of fear of falling (also known as "trepidante Abasie"20,21 or staso-baso-phobia22,23).

    Several forms of apraxia have been reported in patients with Alzheimer’s disease,24–27 though walking problems and trunk movement deficits have been underinvestigated in this condition.10

    Our aim in the present study was to verify whether such patients show any walking disorders that cannot be accounted for by the causes listed above. These disorders would be interpreted as resulting from gait apraxia. A secondary aim was to investigate the relation between gait apraxia and limb apraxia.

    METHODS

    Participants

    Participants were selected from among patients with Alzheimer’s disease who did not present with any concomitant known disorder that could affect their walking. Six degenerative diseases were targeted for exclusion, using formal diagnostic criteria: dementia with Lewy bodies,28 idiopathic Parkinson’s disease,29 progressive supranuclear palsy,30 corticobasal degeneration,31 frontotemporal dementia,32 and vascular dementia.33

    The patients considered for the study were drawn from a consecutive series of 89 patients referred to the dementia neuropsychology unit of the neurology department, University of Milan, San Paolo Hospital, with a diagnosis of progressive cognitive deterioration from possible or probable Alzheimer’s disease (NINCDS-ADRDA criteria34). Twenty four of these were excluded: five were unwilling to participate or were affected by disabling degenerative disease of the hip or knee joints; 11 had extrapyramidal signs (for example, resting tremor, rigidity) severe enough to suggest a concomitant extrapyramidal disease; and eight had hallucinations or were under treatment with neuroleptic drugs. The two step exclusion procedure (exclusion of patients with other dementias and with concomitant disorders) considerably lowered the likelihood that the patients included in our sample were affected by extrapyramidal symptoms or non-Alzheimer dementias.

    The final study cohort comprised 65 outpatients (16 men, 49 women). Three of these were subsequently excluded from analysis because it became apparent during debriefing that they had a poor understanding or recollection of the task instructions. Two further patients were excluded because they did not complete the test, leaving a final study population of 60 patients. Twenty one (35%) of these 60 patients entering the final analyses were less than 70 years old. All had a structural brain scan with either computed tomography (CT) (46 cases) or magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) (14 cases); these had been carried out to exclude the presence of tumour or large vascular lesions. Small vascular lacunae were detected by CT in two patients and by MRI in five others; these seven patients were included in the study as their clinical history and disease progression were strongly suggestive of Alzheimer’s disease.35

    The severity of cognitive deterioration was evaluated by a neuropsychologically oriented psychometric severity scale (the Milan overall dementia assessment (MODA)36), the score of which ranges from 0 (worst score) to 100 (cut off score = 85.5/100, which corresponds to the outer tolerance limit in the normal population). Raw MODA scores are adjusted to account for the effect of age and education. This dementia assessment tool correlates highly (r = 0.87) with the mini-mental state examination36,37 and was chosen because it offers more extensive testing of cognitive abilities.

    Demographic and clinical features of the sample are summarised in table 1A; the median for the MODA was 70.5. From the clinical notes taken at the time of the first examination, it appears that gait problems (or problems in rising from a chair or turning in bed) were prominent in nine of the 60 patients (15%) included in the study (one of these had neuroradiological signs of white matter changes). The demographic features of this subgroup of nine patients are given in table 1B.

    Table 1 Demographical features and dementia severity of (A) the 60 patients with Alzheimer’s disease included in this study, and (B) the nine patients who formed a subgroup with walking difficulties at the initial clinical examination

    Assessment of walking skills

    A new test was devised to assess walking and related movements. In the selection of the items composing this test, the multiple features characterising gait apraxia11,38 were considered. Zingerle39 categorised these into four groups:

    inappropriate righting and supporting reactions when going from lying and sitting to the upright position;

    disequilibrium while walking;

    unnecessary and superfluous (literally "parasitic"40) or dyskinetic movements;

    locomotor disorders including gait initiation failure,14,41 irregularities of rhythm, size, speed, and direction of steps, uncoordinated movements of trunk, lower limbs, and upper arm swinging, and inability to negotiate turns.

    In summary, gait apraxia causes clumsy and bizarre trunk and leg movements, leading to ineffective propulsion. Hence, both the gait itself and the trunk movements were considered in the assessment of walking skills. As with all tests assessing apraxia, this instrument is not diagnostic either for the underlying disease or for gait apraxia. Once all other potential causes of impairment of walking have been excluded, performance on the assessment of walking skills is taken as a measure of gait apraxia.

    Characteristics of the assessment of walking skills

    The assessment of walking skills (AWS) included 42 items (plus four items used as run-in examples) devised to assess trunk and gait, partly derived from earlier work13,14,38,39,41 and partly devised de novo. The items included in the final version of the test were selected through a series of pilot studies and were grouped into two components: assessment of trunk movements (22 items) and walking component proper (20 items). The total score for walking skills ranged from zero to a best of 42. Practice items were available for the two individual components of the test. A list of items for the two components of the assessment of walking skills is given in the appendix.

    Assessment of administration and scoring procedures for the AWS

    Instructions were standardised. Each item was demonstrated and verbally described by the examiner. The participants had to respond by imitation immediately after presentation. This procedure was employed in order to minimise potential confusion between apraxic and verbal comprehension errors. Two examiners were present, one demonstrating the items and the other observing, interacting, and recording the patient’s performance. Participants were debriefed at the end of the session so as to reassure the examiner that they had understood and remembered the rules of the task. Administration time was approximately 40 minutes. An item was scored as pass or fail according to a set of rules derived from previous studies of apraxia.42 An item was considered as failed when performance was clearly inaccurate, such as crossing the legs while attempting to walk,14 or when the performance was hampered by the inability to keep an upright stance,40 accompanied by parasitic arm and trunk movements or bizarre leg movements, or impeded by impaired balance (for example, "déséquilibration frontale"—frontal imbalance43). Items were also considered as failed when the general organisation of the movements was relatively preserved but their execution lacked dexterity (for example, "eigentümliche Ungeschicklichkeit"44—overwhelming lack of dexterity; "bewegunstechnische Schwierigkiet"17—halting movement execution; "Tapsigkeit"45 or "Unbeholfenheit"46—awkwardness). An item generating no response after several solicitations was also scored as a fail.

    Normative data were gathered from a sample of 182 healthy volunteers (123 women and 59 men; mean (SD) age 56 (21) years, range 20 to 92; 96 were Italian and 86 British). Forty one of the controls entering the study (10 men, 31 women) were retested by a different examiner after five to seven days. As expected in a test assessing gait and related movements, the distribution of scores in healthy controls was pronouncedly skewed towards the top score (median 42; observed range, 30 to 42) and age was not relevant (table 2), although the variability increased in individuals older than 70 years. Inner and outer tolerance limits were computed by means of a non-parametric procedure.47 The outer tolerance limit defines the score at which, or below which, the probability that an individual belongs to the normal population is less than 5%. The outer tolerance limit (that is, 38) was used as cut off score. The test–retest reliability was 0.98.

    Table 2 Mean performance on the assessment of walking skills achieved by the 182 healthy controls subdivided according to age decade

    Upper limb apraxia assessment

    For upper limb assessment, we used the upper limb apraxia test, a test assessing ideomotor apraxia devised by De Renzi et al.48 This is a 24 item imitation test consisting of half meaningful and half meaningless movements, half requiring to hold a posture and half the execution of a motor sequence. The total score ranges from zero to a best of 72. Participants were tested with their dominant hand. Fifty eight of the participating Alzheimer patients were tested with this task.

    RESULTS

    The mean (SD) score of the Alzheimer patients in the AWS was 37.33 (5.86), range 17 to 42. Figure 1 shows the distribution of performances on the AWS score in these patients. Thirty six (60%) of the patients scored above cut off. The performance of the remaining 24 patients (40%) was scattered across the range of possible scores, with a large proportion of the patients (30%) performing just below cut off (two of whom had evidence of white matter changes), while the remainder performed poorly. All the nine patients presenting with clinical signs of walking difficulties performed well below cut off in the AWS, their scores being among the lowest observed in the Alzheimer group. Their average score (27.3) differed significantly from that of the other 51 patients entering the study (39.6) (F1,58 = 118.724, p<0.0001).

    Figure 1 Distribution of patients with Alzheimer’s disease according to the assessment of walking skills score.

    Poor performance on the AWS by the Alzheimer group could not be attributed to the number of non-responses to certain items, as from 2520 possible responses (42 items by 60 participants), there was only a negligible proportion of non-responses (136, or 5.4%). Moreover, the distribution of correct responses shows that the Alzheimer patients failed most items evenly, indicating that their poor performance cannot be ascribed to a cluster of particularly difficult items. Only two items (items 21 and 28 in the Trunk component) were passed by all patients, and only one item (item 20 in the Walking component) was failed by more than 40% of the Alzheimer group). A t test analysis was run to compare the two parts of the test. There was no statistical difference between the two sets of items (t40 = -1.64, p = 0.10).

    The Spearman correlation coefficient between dementia severity measured by the MODA and the AWS score was 0.47 (p<0.0001). Indeed, only six of 29 Alzheimer patients with mild dementia (MODA score above 70) performed below the cut off score (21%), compared with 16 of 31 (52%) of those with moderate to severe dementia.

    To evaluate whether the nine patients reporting walking difficulties on clinical examination had these problems because of the severity of their dementia, a correlation analysis was run between their MODA scores and their AWS scores. No significant correlation was found (r = 0.046, p = 0.89).

    Twenty two of the 58 Alzheimer patients tested with the upper limb apraxia test performed below cut off (38%); the correlation between the limb apraxia test and the measure of dementia severity was 0.39 (p<0.005). The correlation between the AWS scores and performance in the upper limb apraxia test was significant, at 0.50 (p<0.0001). No correlation between the AWS scores and the upper limb apraxia scores was observed in the subgroup of nine patients who reported walking difficulties during the clinical examination (r = -0.30, p = 0.37). A stepwise regression procedure on the AWS scores, with age, dementia severity, and limb apraxia scores entered as independent variables, showed that all three were significant independent predictors of walking performance (r = 0.23, 0.53, and 0.50, respectively; p<0.0001 in all cases).

    DISCUSSION

    Gait disturbances are not uncommon in Alzheimer’s disease and have been observed also in Alzheimer patients carefully selected to be exempt from extrapyramidal, ataxic, and paretic signs or clinically relevant musculoskeletal impairments.3 Patients with Alzheimer’s disease are at increased risk of losing their balance3,12 and falling compared with matched controls.4,19,49 Various studies have reported very different figures for prevalence of gait abnormalities, ranging from 8.7%50 to over 90%,51 because of different inclusion criteria and different assessment procedures.

    The lack of a standardised instrument to assess gait has been implicated as a possible cause for the low frequency of reports.2 In this paper, a new test assessing walking skills was devised and norms collected to calculate cut off scores with population based inferential values. A large group of patients with Alzheimer’s disease was tested with this new instrument, and a sizeable proportion (40%) scored below cut off, showing some deficit in their walking skills; a smaller proportion was severely impaired. These figures contrast with the 15% detected by simple clinical observation. Indeed the use of a standardised instrument has been openly advocated.52 Although the possibility of right–left confusion, working memory deficits, and problems with verbal comprehension was minimised by demonstrating the items, the complexity of some of them might have contributed to inflating the proportion of patients performing poorly. It is important to note, however, that items were scored as failed only when clear apraxic errors were observed.

    The assessment of walking skills on its own cannot discriminate between walking disorders caused by gait apraxia and other neurological causes of walking difficulty. Thus to advocate the presence of gait apraxia, alternative causes of walking abnormalities in Alzheimer’s disease have to be excluded for a start. The Alzheimer patients enrolled in this study had no overt extrapyramidal impairments or other concurrent neurological diseases. The presence of associated vascular pathology in a few patients also cannot account for the outcome. Neuroradiological signs of white matter changes were reported in only three of the 24 patients who scored below cut off in the assessment of walking skills. As there are no plausible alternative interpretations that might account for the walking deficits observed in this highly selected sample of Alzheimer patients, by exclusion they are interpreted as resulting from gait apraxia.

    One third of the Alzheimer patients performed below cut off on the upper limb apraxia test. This percentage is remarkably similar to those reported in previous studies.24,26 The AWS scores correlate highly with the scores achieved in the ideomotor apraxia test. The high correlation between the two sets of scores—which is not removed when dementia severity is considered—indicates an association between the two functions. This opposes the argument that gait apraxia should not be considered a true form of apraxia because of dissociation from limb apraxia53 (for a discussion on double dissociations, see Dunn and Kirsner54). Generally, gait apraxia refers to deficits of a relatively unitary function with no reference to a theoretical model. However, some investigators have classed gait apraxia among the melokinetic apraxias55,56—gliedkinetische Apraxie57 or innervatorische Apraxie58—owing to the lack of hemispheric dominance and the characteristic clumsiness. Gait apraxia does not belong to the realm of ideational deficits—that is, patients with gait apraxia know what walking entails. Within the dichotomy proposed by Benke59 between a conceptual system of motor acts and a system controlling sensorimotor and spatiotemporal features of movement, gait apraxia would arise from impairment of the latter system. Gait apraxia, which affects the performance of highly routinised synergistic actions, should also be distinguished from ideomotor apraxia, which hampers individual movements and meaningless gestures. Alzheimer himself suggested a possible dissociation between the presence of upper limb apraxia and the absence of gait disturbances in his patient Auguste D.60

    Gait apraxia should be considered as a possible cause of walking difficulties when there are walking abnormalities that cannot otherwise be accounted for in patients with moderate Alzheimer’s disease, or indeed other dementias.52 Walking disorders have previously been attributed to gait apraxia in a few single case reports of patients suffering from cortical degenerative diseases (table 3). Kurlan et al,10 in their review of gait disorders in Alzheimer’s disease, attributed these deficits to gait apraxia. Gait apraxia was also suggested as the cause of walking impairment in the two demented patients reported by Rossor et al.61 In one of those patients, signs of Alzheimer’s disease pathology were found at necropsy, and both patients had clear medial frontal hypometabolism detected with positron emission tomography. The association between gait apraxia and mesial frontal lesions is in agreement with the body of observations of patients with stroke or tumour (for a review see Della Sala et al62).

    Table 3 Published reports of cases of gait apraxia in patients with degenerative and progressive paralysis caused by cerebral diseases

    In the current study, gait apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease was associated with dementia severity. This ties in with the common clinical observation that severely affected patients may have great difficulty in walking,2,12,66 are stiff and clumsy in performing trunk movements, and when bedridden, are often hardly able to turn over in their bed. Gait apraxia is considered a sequel of mesial bilateral frontal damage.62,67 The observation of a clear correlation between AWS scores and the severity of dementia supports the hypothesis that gait apraxia may emerge when damage to the frontal areas of the brain appears later in the course of the disease. The progression of the neuronal degenerative process in Alzheimer’s disease is maintained to follow a schematic three step pattern.11,18,68,69 The temporomedial encroachment is followed by the temporoparietal neocortical and, later, prefrontal involvement (reviewed by Spinnler70). Deterioration of performance on the MODA parallels this progression.71 Thus it should not be surprising that the proportion of Alzheimer patients failing the AWS test increases with increasing severity, when the degenerative process is likely to involve the frontal lobes more extensively. This may not necessarily be the case, however, as shown by the lack of a significant correlation between the MODA and AWS scores in the patients who, on initial clinical examination, reported walking difficulties as a major symptom that were not accounted for by any other neurological disorder. These patients were the ones among the group who achieved the lowest scores on the AWS test.

    Conclusions

    Our study has shown first, that walking disorders in patients with Alzheimer’s disease may result from gait apraxia; second, that the detection of gait apraxia is eased by the use of a standardised test, but still relies heavily on the exclusion of other causes of walking deficits; and third, that gait apraxia is a recognisable and independent form of apraxia.

    APPENDIX

    Items contained in each component of the AWS test

    Walking

    Examples: Walk with right hand on stomach and left hand on back

    Walk with left hand on stomach and right hand on back

    1 Walk on the spot

    2 Walk forward

    3 Walk with a wide stance (legs apart)

    4 Walk to the right while facing forward

    5 Walk crossing your legs over

    6 Walk backwards

    7 Walk with your feet diverging (pointing out)

    8 Walk to the left while facing forward

    9 Walk on your heels

    10 Walk with feet converging (pointing in)

    11 Walk with your trunk laterally bent to the left

    12 Walk with your left arm stretched out and your right arm hanging by your side

    13 Walk with arms folded

    14 Walk with your trunk bent forward

    15 Walk with your right arm stretched out and your left arm hanging by your side

    16 Walk with your trunk laterally bent to the right

    17 Walk with your hands behind the nape of your neck

    18 Walk with your arms still alongside your trunk

    19 Walk with your trunk extended backwards

    20 Walk stepping over an imaginary obstacle

    Trunk

    Examples: Sit down (on the right edge of the bed)

    Sit on the edge of the bed

    21 Anterior flexion of head

    22 Rotate head to the left

    23 Right lateral flexion

    24 Posterior flexion

    25 Rotate head to the right

    26 Left lateral flexion

    27 Anterior flexion of trunk

    28 Shrug shoulders

    29 Hold stomach in

    30 Left lateral flexion of trunk

    31 Posterior flexion of trunk

    32 Right anterior flexion

    33 Push stomach out

    34 Sit down

    35 Lie supine (starting from the right side of the bed)

    36 Lie prone (starting from the right side of the bed)

    37 Turn from prone to supine

    38 Turn from supine to prone

    39 Prone, get up

    40 Lie supine (starting from the left side of the bed)

    41 While supine, hug both knees

    42 Lie prone (starting from the left edge of the bed)

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    We thank G J Capone, D Costato, and A T Wells for their help in running the pilot studies and collecting part of the normative data. We are also grateful to Ian Deregowski who translated the paper by Rataj and Korohoda from Polish.

    REFERENCES

    Adams K, Victor M, Ropper AH. Principles of neurology, 6th ed. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1997:1052.

    Ala TA, Frey WH. Validation of the NINCDS-ADRDA criteria regarding gait in the clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer disease. A clinicopathologic study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord 1995;9:152–9.

    Alexander NB, Mollo JM, Giordani B, et al. Maintenance of balance, gait patterns, and obstacle clearance in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1995;45:908–14.

    Morris JC, Rubin EH, Morris EJ, et al. Senile dementia of the Alzheimer’s type: an important risk factor for serious falls. J Gerontol 1987;42:412–17.

    Camicioli R, Howieson D, Lehman S, et al. Talking while walking: the effect of a dual task in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology 1997;48:955–8.

    Cocchini G, Della Sala S, Logie RH, et al. Dual task effects of walking while talking in Alzheimer disease. Rev Neurol (in press).

    Kindron D, Freedman M. Motor functioning. In: Morris RG, ed. The cognitive neuropsychology of Alzheimer-type dementia. New York: Oxford University Press, 1996:206–20.

    Burns A, Jacoby R, Levy R. Neurological signs in Alzheimer’s disease. Age Ageing 1991;20:45–51.

    Girling D, Berrios G. Extrapyramidal signs, primitive reflexes and frontal lobe function in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type. Br J Psychiatry 1990;157:888–93.

    Kurlan R, Richard IH, Papka M, et al. Movement disorders in Alzheimer’s disease: More rigidity of definition is needed. Move Disord 2000;15:24–9.

    Nutt JG, Marsden CD, Thomson PD. Human walking and higher-level gait disorders, particularly in the elderly. Neurology 1993;43:268–79.

    Visser H. Gait and balance in senile dementia of Alzheimer’s type. Age Ageing 1983;12:296–301.

    Mayer YS, Barron DW. Apraxia of gait: clinico-physiological study. Brain 1960;83:261–84.

    Gerstmann J, Schilder P. über eine besondere Gangst?rung bei Stirnhirnerkrankung. Wiener Medizinische Wochenschrift 1926;76:97–102.

    Sudarsky L, Ronthal M. Gait disorders among elderly patients: a survey study of 50 patients. Arch Neurol 1983;40:740–3.

    Tyrrell PJ. Apraxia of gait of higher level gait disorders: review of two cases of progressive gait disturbance due to frontal lobe degeneration. J R Soc Med 1994;87:454–6.

    Kleist K. Kortikale (innervatorische) Apraxie. Jahrbuch der Psychiatrie 1907;28:46–112.

    Nutt JG. Gait and balance. In: Goetz CG, Pappert EJ, eds. Textbook of clinical neurology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1999:301–14.

    Buchner DM, Larson EB. Falls and fractures in patients with Alzheimer-type dementia. JAMA 1987;257:1492–5.

    Petren K. über den Zusammenhang zwischen anatomisch bedingter und funkioneller Gangst?rung (besonders in der Form von trepidanter Abasie) im Greisenalter, 1. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 1901;33:818–71.

    Petren K. über den Zusammenhang zwischen anatomisch bedingter und funkioneller Gangst?rung (besonders in der Form von trepidanter Abasie) im Greisenalter, 2. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 1902;34:444–89.

    Garcin R. The ataxias. In: Vinken PJ, Bruyn GW, eds. Handbook of clinical neurology Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1969:309–52.

    Murphy J, Isaacs B. The post-fall syndrome: a study of 36 elderly patients. Gerontology 1982;82:265–70.

    Della Sala S, Lucchelli F, Spinnler H. Ideomotor apraxia in patients with dementia of Alzheimer type. J Neurol 1987;234:91–3.

    Derouesné C, Lagha-Pierucci S, Thibault S, et al. Apraxia disturbances in patients with mild to moderate Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychologia 2000;38:1760–9.

    Edwards DF, Deul RK, Baum CM, et al. Quantitative analysis of apraxia in senile dementia of the Alzheimer type: stage-related differences in prevalence and type. Dementia 1991;2:142–9.

    Capone JG, Della Sala S, Spinnler H, et al. Upper and lower face and ideomotor apraxia in patients with Alzheimer’s disease. Behav Neurol 2003;14:1–8.

    McKeith IG, Galasko D, Kosaka K, et al. Consensus guidelines for the clinical and pathologic diagnosis of dementia with Lewy bodies (DBL): report of the consortium on DBL international workshop. Neurology 1996;47:1113–24.

    Jankovic J, Stacy M. Movement disorders. In: Goetz CG, Pappert EJ, eds. Textbook of clinical neurology. Philadelphia: WB Saunders, 1999:655–79.

    Litvan I, Cambell G, Mangone CA, et al. Which clinical features differentiate progressive supranuclear palsy (Steel-Richardson-Olszewski syndrome) from related disorders? Brain 1997;120:65–74.

    Gibb WRG, Luthert PJ, Marsden CD. Corticobasal degeneration. Brain 1989;112:1171–92.

    Snowden JS, Neary D, Mann DMA. Frontotemporal lobar degeneration: frontotemporal dementia, progressive aphasia, semantic dementia. London: Churchill Livingstone, 1996.

    Chui HC, Victoroff JI, Margolin D, et al. Criteria for the diagnosis of ischemic vascular dementia proposed by the State of California’s Alzheimer’s disease diagnostic and treatment centers. Neurology 1992;42:473–80.

    McKhann G, Drachman D, Folstein M, et al. Clinical diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: report of the NINCDA-ADRDA Work Group under the auspices of Department of Health and Human Services Task Force on Alzheimer’s Disease. Neurology 1984;34:939–44.

    Esiri MM. Which vascular lesions are of importance in vascular dementia? Ann NY Acad Sci 2000;903:239–43.

    Brazzelli M, Capitani E, Della Sala S, et al. A neuropsychological instrument adding to the description of patients with suspected cortical dementia: the Milan overall dementia assessment. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1994;57:1510–17.

    Cazzaniga R. Relationship between MODA and MMSE in the evaluation of dementia severity. Milan: University of Milan, 2002; .

    Marsden CD, Thompson PD. Toward a nosology of gait disorders: descriptive classification. In: Masdeu JC, Sudarsky L, Wolfson L, eds. Gait disorders of aging Philadelphia: Lippincott-Raven, 1997:135–57.

    Zingerle H. Ausfalls-Syndrom der frontalen Brücken-Kleinhirn-Bahnen (Tractus fronto-ponto-cerebellaris). Zeitschrift für die gesamte Psychiatrie une Neurologie 1934;149:737–64.

    P?tzl O. über ein neuartiges Syndrome bei Hirnerkrankungen des Stirnhirnpoles (Neurologischer Bericht über einen der Elschnigschen F?lle von erfolgreich operiertem Stirnhirnabscess). Zeitschrift für die gesamte Psychiatrie une Neurologie 1924;91:147–82.

    Atchison PR, Thomson PD, Frackowiak RSJ, et al. The syndrome of gait ignition failure: a report of six cases. Move Disord 1993;8:285–92.

    Bizzozero I, Costato D, Della Sala S, et al. Upper and lower face apraxia: role of the right hemisphere. Brain 2000;123:2213–30.

    van Bogaert L, Martin P. Sur deux signes du syndrome de déséquilibration frontale. L’apraxie de la marche et l’atonie statique. L’Encéphale 1929;24:11–18.

    Liepmann H. über St?rungen des Handelns. Berlin: Karger, 1905.

    Poppelreuter W. Die psychischen Sch?dingungen durch Kopfschu? im Kriege 1914–1916, vol 1: Die St?rungen der niederen und h?heren Sehleistungen durch Verletzung des Okzipitalhirns. Leipzig: Voss., Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1991.

    Kleist K. Gehirnpathologie, vornehmlich auf Grund der Kriegserfahrungen. Leipzig: Barth, 1934.

    Ackermann H. Mehrdimensionale nicht-parametrische Normbereiche. Metodologische und medizinische Aspekte.. Berlin: Springer-Verlag, 1985.

    De Renzi E, Motti F, Nichelli P. Imitating gestures: a quantitative approach to ideomotor apraxia. Arch Neurol 1980;37:6–10.

    van Dijk PTM, Meulenberg OGR, van de Sande HJ, et al. Falls in dementia patients. Gerontologist 1993;33:200–4.

    Koller WC, Wilson RS, Glatt SL, et al. Motor signs are infrequent in dementia of the Alzheimer type. Ann Neurol 1984;16:514–16.

    Fransenn EH, Kluger A, Torossian CL, et al. The neurologic syndrome of severe Alzheimer’s disease. Relationship to functional decline. Arch Neurol 1993;50:1029–39.

    Verghese J, Lipton RB, Hall CB, et al. Abnormalities of gait as a predictor of non-Alzheimer’s dementia. N Engl J Med 2002;347:1761–8.

    Poeck K. Clues to the nature of disruptions to limb apraxia. In: Roy EA, ed. Neuropsychological studies of apraxia. Amsterdam: North-Holland, 1985:99–109.

    Dunn JC, Kirsner K. What can we infer from double dissociations? Cortex 2003;39:1–8.

    Liepmann H. Drei Aufs?tze aus dem Apraxiegebiet. Berlin: Karger, 1908.

    Petrovici I. Apraxia of gait and trunk movements. J Neurol Sci 1968;7:229–43.

    Westphal K. über einen Fall von motorische Apraxie. Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie 1907;64:452–9.

    Kleist K. Kortikale (innervatorische) Apraxie. Jahrbuch der Psychiatrie 1907;28:46–112.

    Benke T. Two forms of apraxia in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex 1993;29:715–29.

    Alzheimer A. über eine eigenartige Erkrankung der Hirnrinde . Allgemeine Zeitschrift für Psychiatrie und Psychisch-Gerichtliche Medizin 1907;64:146–8.

    Rossor MN, Tyrrell PJ, Warrington EK, et al. Progressive frontal gait disturbance with atypical Alzheimer’s disease and corticobasal degeneration. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 1999;67:345–52.

    Della Sala S, Francescani A, Spinnler H. Gait apraxia following bilateral SMA lesion. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 2002;72:77–85.

    Bonfiglio F. Di speciali reperti in un caso di probabile sifilide cerebrale. Rivista Sperimentale di Freniatria e Medicina Legale delle Alienazioni Mentali 1908;34:196–206.

    Lange J. Agnosie und Apraxien. In: Bumke O, F?rster O, eds. Handbuch der Neurologie: Allgemeine Neurologie: Gro?hirn, Vegetatives Nervensystem, K?rperbau und Kostitution. Berlin: Springer, 1936:807–960.

    Rataj R, Korohoda J. Niezwykla odmiana apraksji: apraksja tolowia. Neurologia I Neurochirurgia Polska 1969;3:539–41.

    Seltzer B, Sherwin I. A comparison of clinical features in early- and late-onset primary degenerative dementia. One entity or two? Arch Neurol 1983;40:143–6.

    Boccardi E, Della Sala S, Motto C, et al. Utilisation behaviour consequent to bilateral SMA softening. Cortex38:289–308.

    Braak H, Braak E. Neuropathological staging of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol 1991;82:239–59.

    Perry RJ, Watson P, Hodges JR. The nature and staging of attention dysfunction in early (minimal and mild) Alzheimer’s disease: relationship to episodic and semantic memory impairment. Neuropsychologia 2000;38:252–71.

    Spinnler H. Alzheimer’s disease. In: Denes G, Pizzamiglio L, eds. Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology. Hove: Psychology Press, 1999:699–746.

    Capitani E, Manzoni L, Spinnler H. Follow-up of 53 Alzheimer’s patients with the MODA (Milan Overall Dementia Assessment). Eur J Neurol 1997;4:237–9.(S Della Sala1,*, H Spinnl)