当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《国际神经病学神经外科学杂志》 > 2004年第7期 > 正文
编号:11356521
The cognitive neuroscience of autism
http://www.100md.com 《神经病学神经外科学杂志》
     Correspondence to:

    S Baron-Cohen

    Autism Research Centre, Departments of Experimental Psychology and Psychiatry, University of Cambridge, Douglas House, Cambridge CB2 2AH, UK; sb205@cam.ac.uk

    The psychology and biology of a complex developmental condition

    Abbreviations: AS, Asperger syndrome; CC, central coherence; EFT, embedded figures task; E-S, empathising-systemising; HFA, high functioning autism; MRI, magnetic resonance imaging

    Keywords: autism

    Autism is diagnosed when a child or adult has abnormalities in a "triad" of behavioural domains: social development, communication, and repetitive behaviour/obsessive interests.1,2 Autism can occur at any point on the IQ continuum, and IQ is a strong predictor of outcome.3 Autism is also invariably accompanied by language delay (no single words before 2 years old). Asperger syndrome (AS)4 is a subgroup on the autistic spectrum. People with AS share many of the same features as are seen in autism, but with no history of language delay and with an IQ in the average range or above. In this editorial, the main cognitive theories of autism are summarised. These are then followed by a summary of the key neurobiological findings.

    AUTISM: COGNITIVE ASPECTS

    The mind blindness theory of autism5 proposed that in autism spectrum conditions there are deficits in the normal process of empathy, relative to mental age. These deficits can occur by degrees. The term "empathising" encompasses a range of other terms: "theory of mind", "mind reading", "empathy", and taking the "intentional stance".6 Empathy involves two major elements: (1) the ability to attribute mental states to oneself and others, as a natural way to make sense of agents,7–9 and (2) having an emotional reaction that is appropriate to the other person’s mental state (such as sympathy).

    Since the first test of mind blindness in children with autism,10 there have been more than 30 experimental tests. The vast majority of these have revealed profound impairments in the development of their empathising ability. These are reviewed elsewhere.5,11 Some children and adults with AS only show their empathising deficits on age appropriate adult tests.12–14 This deficit in their empathising is thought to underlie the difficulties such children have in social and communicative development,15,16 and in the imagination of others’ minds.17,18 We can think of these symptoms as the triad of deficits (see fig 1).

    Figure 1 The triad of impairments in autism

    Systemising is the drive to analyse systems, in order to understand and predict the behaviour of inanimate events. Systems are all around us in our environment, and include technical systems (such as machines and tools); natural systems (such as biological and geographical phenomena); abstract systems (such as mathematics or computer programs). The way we make sense of any of these systems is in terms of underlying rules and regularities, or specifically an analysis of input-operation-output relations.19 The empathising-systemising (E-S) theory holds that alongside the empathising deficits in autism (see above), systemising is either intact or superior.20 Studies suggest systemising in autism is at least in line with mental age, or superior.21–25 Systemising may relate to a different set of features which we can think of as the triad of strengths (see fig 2).

    Figure 2 The triad of strengths in autism

    People with autism spectrum conditions show unusually strong repetitive behaviour, a strong desire for routines, and a "need for sameness". One cognitive account of this aspect of the syndrome is the executive dysfunction theory.26–28 This assumes that autism involves a form of frontal lobe pathology leading to persevering or inability to shift attention. There is some evidence for such executive deficits.29 But the fact that it is possible for people with AS to exist who have no demonstrable executive dysfunction while still having deficits in empathising and talents in systemising,30 suggests that executive dysfunction is unlikely to be a core feature of autism spectrum conditions.

    The executive account has also traditionally ignored the content of "repetitive behaviour". The E-S theory in contrast draws attention to the fact that much repetitive behaviour involves the child’s "obsessional" or strong interests with mechanical systems (such as light switches or water faucets) or other systems that can be understood in terms of rules and regularities. Rather than these behaviours being a sign of executive dysfunction, these may reflect the child’s intact or even superior interest in systems. One study suggests that autistic obsessions are not random with respect to content (which would be predicted by the content free executive dysfunction theory), but that these test to cluster in the domain of systems.31

    Weak central coherence (CC)32,33 refers to the individual’s preference for local detail over global processing. This has been demonstrated in terms of an autistic superiority on the embedded figures task (EFT) and the block design subtest.25,34,35 It has also been shown in terms of an autistic deficit in integrating fragments of objects and integrating sentences within a paragraph.36 The faster and more accurate performance on the EFT and block design test have been interpreted as evidence of good segmentation skills, and superior attention to detail. The latter has also been demonstrated on visual search tasks.37,38

    Systemising requires excellent attention to detail, identifying parameters that may then be tested for their role in the behaviour of the system under examination. So, both the E-S theory and the CC theory predict excellent attention to detail. However, the E-S and CC theories also make opposite predictions when it comes to an individual with autism being able to understand a whole system. The E-S theory predicts that a person with autism, faced with a new system to learn, will show a stronger drive to learn the system compared with someone without autism, so long as there are underlying rules and regularities that can be discovered. Moreover, they will readily grasp that a change of one parameter in one part of the system may have distant effects on another part of the system. In contrast, the CC theory predicts that they should fail to understand whole (global) systems or the relation between parts of a system. This has not yet been tested.

    AUTISM: NEUROBIOLOGICAL ASPECTS

    Neuroanatomy and neuropathology

    Anatomical abnormalities have been identified in many brain areas in autism. These include the cerebellum,39–42 the brain stem,42,43 frontal lobes,44–47 parietal lobes,48 hippocampus,49,50 and the amygdale.49 Epilepsy also occurs commonly, at least in classic autism.51 In terms of neuropathology, the number of Purkinje cells in the cerebellar cortex is abnormally low.52–55 This has been postulated to lead to disinhibition of the cerebellar deep nuclei and consequent overexcitement of the thalamus and cerebral cortex.56 Abnormalities in the density of packing of neurons in the hippocampus, amygdala, and other parts of the limbic system have also been reported.54,55,57 An abnormally low degree of dendritic branching was also found in a Golgi analysis of the hippocampus of two autistic brains,57 though it remains to be seen if such an abnormality is confirmed in a larger sample. A separate report suggests a reduction in the size of cortical minicolumns and an increase in cell dispersion within these minicolumns. These might indicate an increase in the number of and connectivity between minicolumns.58,59

    Neurophysiology

    Hyper arousal in response to sensory input, and decreased ability to select between competing sensory inputs, has been reported.60,61 Functional neuroimaging suggests increased activity in sensory areas of the brain normally associated with stimulus driven processing, and decreased activity in areas normally associated with higher cognitive processing. Thus, on the EFT, people with autism show unusually high activation in ventral occipital areas and abnormally low activation in prefrontal and parietal areas.62 In one study they also failed to show normal activity in the fusiform "face area",63 instead showing abnormally high activity in the peristriate cortex and inferior temporal gyrus.64,65 The visual N2 to novel stimuli is also heightened to irrelevant stimuli.66 The P3 in response to auditory stimuli is abnormally generalised to occipital sites in visual cortex.67

    Regarding EEG results, the P1 evoked potential is either abnormally heightened in response to stimuli that are the target of attention, or abnormally generalised to stimuli that are outside the target of attention.68 Both hemispheres show abnormal activation—indiscriminately—during shifts of attention into either hemifield.69,70 Regarding attention research, a deficit has been found in rapid shifting of attention between modalities,39 between spatial locations69,71–76 and between object features.77,78

    Morphometry

    Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) morphometry shows volume deficits in the cerebellum,40–42,79 the brainstem,42 and posterior corpus callosum.80 Regarding the cerebellar abnormalities, a subgroup shows increased cerebellar volume.81 A volume deficit has also been reported in the parietal lobe.48 Neuropsychology suggests this is associated with a narrowed spatial focus of attention.68

    Longitudinal morphometry

    Using either MRI volumetric analysis, or measures of head circumference, the autistic brain appears to involve transient postnatal macroencephaly.82 Neonates later diagnosed with autism or PDD-NOS (Pervasive Developmental Disorder-Not Otherwise Specified) have normal head circumference, but by 2–4 years of age 90% of these have MRI based brain volumes larger than average.44–47 This reflects an enlargement of cerebellar and cerebral white matter, and cerebral grey matter.45,83 Enlargement of superficial white matter tracts containing cortico-cortical fibres may persist abnormally late into development, while the internal capsule and corpus callosum are smaller.84 Cerebellar and cerebral white matter volumes, and cerebellar vermis size can distinguish 95% of toddlers with autism from normal controls, and predict if the child with autism will be high or low functioning.45 The overgrowth is anterior to posterior (frontal lobes being the largest). This increase in volume of cortical grey matter may reflect a failure of synaptic pruning, or an excess of synaptogenesis.56

    The "social brain"

    A neural basis of empathy has built on a model first proposed by Brothers.85 She suggested—from animal lesion studies,86 single cell recording studies,87 and neurological studies—that social intelligence was a function of three regions: the amygdala, the orbitofrontal and medial frontal cortex, and the superior temporal sulcus and gyrus (STG). Together, she called these the "social brain". Abnormalities in autism have been found in the amygdala, the orbito and the medial frontal cortex.

    Regarding the amygdala, there are four lines of evidence for an amygdala deficit in autism.88 Firstly, a neuroanatomical study of autism at postmortem found microscopic pathology (in the form of increased cell density) in the amygdala, in the presence of normal amygdala volume.89,90 Secondly, patients with autism tend to show a similar pattern of deficits to those seen in patients with amygdala lesions.91 Thirdly, a recent structural MRI study of autism reported reduced amygdala volume.92 Finally, in a recent functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) study, adults with high functioning autism (HFA) or Asperger syndrome (AS) showed significantly less amygdala activation during a mentalising task (Reading the Mind in the Eyes task) compared with normal.93

    Reduced activity has also been found in the left medial frontal cortex,94 during an empathising (theory of mind) task, and also in the orbitofrontal cortex.95

    GENETICS OF AUTISM SPECTRUM CONDITIONS

    Ultimately, the cognitive and neural abnormalities in autism spectrum conditions are likely to be caused by genetic factors. The sibling risk rate for autism is approximately 4.5%, or a tenfold increase over general population rates.96 In an epidemiological study of same sex autistic twins, it was found that 60% of monozygotic (MZ) pairs were concordant for autism versus no dizygotic, (DZ) pairs.97 When they considered a broader phenotype (of related cognitive or social abnormalities), 92% of MZ pairs were concordant versus 10% of DZ pairs. The high concordance in MZ twins indicated a high degree of genetic influence, and the risk to a co-MZ twin can be estimated at over 200 times the general population rate.

    Molecular genetic studies are beginning to narrow down candidate regions. There is still little consensus, but two regions have been identified in several (but not all) studies. These are 15q11-13, near the GABAA?3 receptor subunit gene (GABRB3) and a second one on 17q11.2, near the serotonin transporter gene (SLC6A4). The latter is of interest because of reports of increased serotonin (5HT) levels of platelets in autism . Serotonin innervates the limbic system, and so plausibly plays a role in emotion recognition and empathy. Mothers homozygous for GABRB3 knockout fail to engage in normal nurturing behaviour and have epileptiform EEG.98,99 At least four loci on the X chromosome have also been implicated in autism, and are of interest for their power to explain the sex ratio in autism (markedly biased towards males). These are the neuroligin genes (NLGN3, NLGN4), FMR1 (which causes fragile X syndrome), and MECP2. Several reviews of the genetics of autism literature are available, but this is a fast changing field.100–102

    As of yet, specific genes for autism have not yet been identified, despite the encouraging possibility of candidate regions on chromosomes. The future of research in this field will be not only to isolate the relevant genes but also to understand the function of these genes, and ultimately the relation between these different causal levels in autism. It is hoped that during this research endeavour there will also be evaluations of the most promising treatments.

    ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

    The author was supported by the MRC during the period of this work. I am grateful to Matthew Belmonte for discussions of the neuroscience of autism literature. This editorial has benefited from a preview of Belmonte et al (in press).

    REFERENCES

    APA. DSM-IV Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th edn. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1994.

    ICD-10. International classification of diseases. 10th edn. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health Organisation, 1994.

    Rutter M . Language disorder and infantile autism. In: Rutter M, Schopler E, eds. Autism: a reappraisal of concepts and treatment. New York: Plenum, 1978.

    Asperger H . Die "Autistischen Psychopathen" im Kindesalter. Archiv fur Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 1944;117:76–136.

    Baron-Cohen S . Mindblindness: an essay on autism and theory of mind. Boston: MIT Press/Bradford Books, 1995.

    Dennett D . The intentional stance. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press/Bradford Books, 1987.

    Baron-Cohen S . How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mindreading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive/Current Psychology of Cognition 1994;13:513–52.

    Leslie A . ToMM, ToBy, and Agency: core architecture and domain specificity. In: Hirschfeld L, Gelman S, eds. Domain specificity in cognition and culture. New York: Cambridge University Press, 1995.

    Premack D . The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition 1990;36:1–16.

    Baron-Cohen S , Leslie AM, Frith U. Does the autistic child have a ‘theory of mind’? Cognition 1985;21:37–46.

    Baron-Cohen S , Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, eds. Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford University Press 1993.

    Baron-Cohen , Joliffe T, Mortimore T, et al. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or Asperger Syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:813–22.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S, Jolliffe T. Is there a "language of the eyes"? Evidence from normal adults and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Visual Cognition 1997;4:311–31.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S, Hill J, et al. The ‘Reading the Mind in the eyes’ test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger Syndrome or High-Functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2001;42:241–52.

    Baron-Cohen S . Social and pragmatic deficits in autism: cognitive or affective? J Autism Dev Disord 1988;18:379–402.

    Tager-Flusberg H . What language reveals about the understanding of minds in children with autism. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ, eds. Understanding other minds: perspectives from autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1993.

    Baron-Cohen S . Autism and symbolic play. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1987;5:139–48.

    Leslie AM. Pretence and representation: the origins of "theory of mind". Psychological Review 1987;94:412–26.

    Baron-Cohen S . The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci 2002;6:248–54.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S, Griffin R, et al. The exact mind: empathising and systemising in autism spectrum conditions. In: Goswami U, ed. Handbook of cognitive development. Oxford: Blackwells, 2002.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S, Scahill V, et al. Are intuitive physics and intuitive psychology independent? Journal of Developmental and Learning Disorders 2001;5:47–78.

    Baron-Cohen S , Richler J, Bisarya D, et al. The Systemising Quotient (SQ): An investigation of adults with Asperger Syndrome or High Functioning Autism and normal sex differences. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2003;358:361–74.

    Baron-Cohen S , Leslie AM, Frith U. Mechanical, behavioural and intentional understanding of picture stories in autistic children. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 1986;4:113–25.

    Lawson J , Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S. Empathising and systemising in adults with and without Asperger Syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord 2004; (in press).

    Jolliffe T , Baron-Cohen S. Are people with autism or Asperger’s Syndrome faster than normal on the Embedded Figures Task? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:527–34.

    Ozonoff S , Rogers S, Farnham J, et al. Can standard measures identify subclinical markers of autism? J Autism Dev Disord 1994;23:429–41.

    Pennington B , Rogers S, Bennetto L, et al. Validity test of the executive dysfunction hypothesis of autism. In: Russell J, ed. Executive functioning in autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1997.

    Russell J . How executive disorders can bring about an inadequate theory of mind. In: Russell J, ed. Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1997.

    Russell J . ed. Autism as an executive disorder. Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1997.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S, Stone V, et al. A mathematician, a physicist, and a computer scientist with Asperger Syndrome: performance on folk psychology and folk physics test. Neurocase 1999;5:475–83.

    Baron-Cohen S , Wheelwright S. Obsessions in children with autism or Asperger Syndrome: a content analysis in terms of core domains of cognition. Br J Psychiatry 1999;175:484–90.

    Frith U . Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell, 1989.

    Happe F . Studying weak central coherence at low levels: children with autism do not succumb to visual illusions. A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996;37:873–7.

    Shah A , Frith U. An islet of ability in autism: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1983;24:613–20.

    Shah A , Frith U. Why do autistic individuals show superior performance on the block design test? J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1993;34:1351–64.

    Jolliffe T , Baron-Cohen S. Linguistic processing in high-functioning adults with autism or Asperger syndrome: Can global coherence be achieved? A further test of central coherence theory. Psychol Med 2000;30:1169–87.

    Plaisted K , O’Riordan M, Baron-Cohen S. Enhanced discrimination of novel, highly similar stimuli by adults with autism during a perceptual learning task. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998;39:765–75.

    Plaisted K , O’Riordan M, Baron-Cohen S. Enhanced visual search for a conjunctive target in autism: A research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1998;39:777–83.

    Courchesne E , Townsend J, Akshoomof NA, et al. Impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behav Neurosci 1994;108:848–65.

    Murakami J , Courchesne E, Press G, et al. Reduced cerebellar hemisphere size and its relationship to vermal hypoplasia in autism. Arch Neurol 1989;46:689–94.

    Courchesne E , Townsend J, Saitoh O. The brain in infantile autism: posterior fossa structures are abnormal. Neurology 1994;44:214–23.

    Hashimoto T , Tayama M, Murakawa K, et al. Development of the brainstem and cerebellum in autistic patients. J Autism Dev Disord 1995;25:1–17.

    Rodier PM, Ingram JL, Tisdale B, et al. Embryological origin for autism: developmental anomalies of the cranial nerve motor nuclei. J Comp Neurol 1996;370:247–61.

    Carper RA, Courchesne E. Inverse correlation between frontal lobe and cerebellum sizes in children with autism. Brain 2000;123:836–44.

    Courchesne E , Karns CM, Davis HR, et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life of patients with autistic disorder. Neurology 2001;57:245–54.

    Sparks BF, Friedman SD, Shaw DW, et al. Brain structural abnormalities in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Neurology 2002;59:184–92.

    Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, et al. Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology 2002;59:175–83.

    Courchesne E , Press GA, Yeung-Courchesne R. Parietal lobe abnormalities detected with MR in patients with infantile autism. AJR Am J Roentgenol 1993;160:387–93.

    Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, et al. MRI volumes of amygdala and hippocampus in non-mentally retarded autistic adolescents and adults. Neurology 1999;53:2145.

    Saitoh O , Karns CM, Courchesne E. Development of hippocampal formation from 2 to 42 years. Brain 2001;124:1317–24.

    Ballaban-Gil K , Tuchman R. Epilepsy and epileptiform EEG: association with autism and language disorders. Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev 2000;6:300–308.

    Williams RS, Hauser SL, Purpura DP, et al. Autism and mental retardation: neuropathologic studies performed in four retarded persons with autistic behaviour. Arch Neurol 1980;37:749–53.

    Ritvo ER, Freeman BJ, Scheibel AB, et al. Lower Purkinje cell counts in the cerebella of four autistic subjects: initial findings of the UCLA-NSAC autopsy research report. Am J Psychiatry 1986;143:862–66.

    Bauman M , Kempner T. Histoanatomic observation of the brain in early infantile autism. Neurology 1985;35:866–74.

    Bauman ML, Kemper TL. Neuroanatomic observations of the brain in autism. In: Bauman ML, Kemper TL, eds. The neurobiology of autism. Baltimore: John Hopkins University Press, 1994:119–45.

    Belmonte MK, et al. Autism as a disorder of neural information processing: directions for research and targets for therapy. Molecular Psychiatry 2004; (in press).

    Raymond G , Bauman M, Kemper T. Hippocampus in autism: a Golgi analysis. Acta Neuropathol 1996;91:117–19.

    Casanova MF, Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, et al. Minicolumnar pathology in autism. Neurology 2002;58:428–32.

    Casanova MF, Buxhoeveden DP, Switala AE, et al. Asperger’s Syndrome and cortical neuropathology. J Child Neurol 2002;17:142–5.

    Tordjman S , Anderson GM, McBride PA, et al. Plasma beta-endorphin, adrenocorticotropin hormone and cortisol in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1997;38:705–15.

    Hirstein W , Iversen P, Ramachandran VS. Autonomic responses of autistic children to people and objects. Proc R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2001;268:1883–8.

    Ring H , Baron-Cohen S, Williams S, et al. Cerebral correlates of preserved cognitive skills in autism. A functional MRI study of Embedded Figures task performance. Brain 1999;122:1305–15.

    Pierce K , Muller R - A, Ambrose J, et al. Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism; evidence from functional MRI. Brain 2001;124:2059–73.

    Critchley HD, Daly EM, Bullmore ET, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour. Brain 2000;123:2203–12.

    Schultz R , Gauthier I, Klin A, et al. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome during face discrimination. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000;57:331–40.

    Kemner C , Verbaten MN, Cuperus JM, et al. Visual and somotosensory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1994;92:225–37.

    Kemner C , Verbaten MN, Cuperus JM, et al. Auditory event-related brain potentials in autistic children and three different control groups. Biol Psychiatry 1995;38:150–65.

    Townsend J , Courchesne E. Parietal damage and narrow "spotlight" spatial attention. J Cogn Neurosci 1994;6:220–32.

    Belmonte MK. Abnormal attention in autism shown by steady-state visual evoked potentials. Autism 2000;4:269–85.

    Belmonte MK, Yurgelun-Todd DA. Functional anatomy of impaired selective attention and compensatory processing in autism. Cognitive Brain Research 2003;17:651–64.

    Wainwright-Sharp JA, Bryson SE. Visual orienting deficits in high-functioning people with autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1993;23:1–13.

    Wainwright-Sharp JA, Bryson SE. Visual-spatial orienting in autism. J Autism Dev Disord 1996;26:423–38.

    Townsend J , Courchesne E, Egaas B. Slowed orienting of covert visual-spatial attention in autism: specific deficits associated with cerebellar and parietal abnormality. Development and Psychopathology 1996;8:563–84.

    Townsend J , Singer-Harris N, Courchesne E. Visual attention abnormalities in autism: delayed orienting to locationi. J International Neuropsychol Soc 1996;2:541–50.

    Townsend J , Courchesne E, Covington J, et al. Spatial attention deficits in patients with acquired or developmental cerebellar abnormality. J Neurosci 1999;19:5632–43.

    Harris NS, Courchesne E, Townsend J, et al. Neuroanatomic contributions to slowed orienting of attention in children with autism. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res 1999;8:61–71.

    Courchesne E , Townsend J, Akshoomoff NA, et al. A new finding: impairment in shifting attention in autistic and cerebellar patients. In: Broman SH, Grafman J, eds. Atypical cognitive deficits in developmental disorders: implications for brain function. Hillsdale, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum, 1994.

    Rinehart NJ, Bradshaw JL, Moss SA, et al. A deficit in shifting attention present in high-functioning autism but not Asperger’s disorder. Autism 2001;5:67–80.

    Courchesne E , Yeung-Courchesne R, Press G, et al. Hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules VI and VII in infantile autism. N Engl J Med 1988;318:1349–54.

    Egaas B , Courchesne E, Saitoh O. Reduced size of corpus callosum in autism. Arch Neurol 1995;52:794–801.

    Courchesne E , Saitoh O, Yeung-Courchesne R, et al. Abnormality of cerebellar vermian lobules VI and VII in patients with infantile autism: Identification of hypoplastic and hyperplastic subgroups with MR imaging. Am J Radiol 1994;162:123–30.

    Courchesne E . Abnormal early brain development in autism. Molecular Psychiatry 2002;7:21–3.

    Herbert MR, Zeigler DA, Deutsch CK, et al. Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain 2003;126:1182–92.

    Herbert MR, Zeigler DA, Makris N, et al. White matter increases in autism are largely in superficial radiate regions. International Meeting for Autism Research, Orlando, Florida 2002.

    Brothers L . The social brain: a project for integrating primate behaviour and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience 1990;1:27–51.

    Kling A , Brothers L. The amygdala and social behavior. In: Aggleton J, ed. Neurobiological aspects of emotion, memory, and mental dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss, Inc, 1992.

    Brothers L , Ring B, Kling A. Responses of neurons in the macaque amygdala to complex social stimuli. Behav Brain Res 1990;41:199–213.

    Baron-Cohen S , Ring H, Bullmore E, et al. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2000;24:355–64.

    Bauman M , Kemper T. The neurobiology of autism. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins, 1994.

    Rapin I , Katzman R. Neurobiology of autism. Ann Neurol 1998;43:7–14.

    Adolphs R , Sears L, Piven J. Abnormal processing of social information from faces in autism. J Cogn Neurosci 2001;13:232–40.

    Abell F , Krams M, Ashburner J, et al. The neuranatomy of autism: a voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Cogn Neurosci 1999;10:1647–51.

    Baron-Cohen S , Ring H, Wheelwright S, et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 1999;11:1891–8.

    Happe F , Ehlers S, Fletcher P, et al. Theory of mind in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger Syndrome. Neuro Report 1996;8:197–201.

    Baron-Cohen S , Ring H, Moriarty J, et al. Recognition of mental state terms: a clinical study of autism, and a functional neuroimaging study of normal adults. Br J Psychiatry 1994;165:640–9.

    Jorde L , Hasstedt S, Ritvo E, et al. Complex segregation analysis of autism. Am J Hum Gen 1991;49:932–8.

    Bailey A , Le Couteur A, Gottesman I, et al. Autism as a strongly genetic disorder: evidence from a British twin study. Psychol Med 1995;25:63–77.

    Homanics GE, DeLorey TM, Firestone LL, et al. Mice devoid of gamma-aminobutyrate type A receptor ?3 subunit have epilepsy, cleft palate and hypersensitive behaviour. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 1997;94:4143–8.

    DeLorey TM, Handforth A, Anagnostaras SG, et al. Mice lacking the ?3 subunit of the GABAa receptor have the epilepsy phenotype and many of the behavioural characteristics of Angelman syndrome. J Neurosci 1998;18:8505–14.

    Cook Jnr EH. Genetics of autism. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am 2001;10:333–50.

    Folstein SE, Rosen-Sheidley B. Genetics of autism: complex aetiology for a heterogeneous disorder. Nat Rev Genet 2001;2:943–55.

    Lauritsen M , Ewald H. The genetics of autism. Acta Psychiatr Scand 2001;103:411–27.(S Baron-Cohen)