当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《国际神经病学神经外科学杂志》 > 2005年第4期 > 正文
编号:11356767
Diseases of the nervous system: patients’ aetiological beliefs
http://www.100md.com 《神经病学神经外科学杂志》
     1 Neurology Department, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Lausanne, Switzerland

    2 Institute of Cellular Biology and Morphology, University of Lausanne, Rue du Bugnon 9, CH-1005 Lausanne, Switzerland

    Correspondence to:

    Dr J Bogousslavsky

    Neurology Department, Centre Hospitalier Universitaire Vaudois (CHUV), Rue du Bugnon 46, 1011 Lausanne, Switzerland; julien.bogousslavsky@chuv.hospvd.ch

    ABSTRACT

    Background: Patients’ opinions about the aetiology of their disease and the implications for compliance have not been well documented at this time.

    Objective: To investigate prospectively aetiological beliefs of a cohort of neurological inpatients.

    Methods: Within two days of admission, patients orally answered a short questionnaire regarding their beliefs about the aetiology of their disease and the possible influence of psychological factors, stress, fatigue, excessive work or other activities, poor lifestyle, conflict with another person, a tragic event, chance, and destiny.

    Results: Of the 342 patients who participated in the study, 49% spontaneously said that they had no idea of what could have caused their disease, 15% gave a congruent medical explanation, 11% mentioned stress and fatigue as a precipitating factor, and 6% evoked a non-congruent medical explanation. Thirty six per cent thought that psychological factors had triggered their disease; such factors being blamed by a higher proportion of young patients and patients with chronic central nervous system diseases. The triggering factors most often blamed were stress (48%, especially by patients with headache), fatigue (51%), chance (54%), and destiny (43%).

    Conclusions: Patients’ aetiological beliefs only partially concur with medical opinion and this may influence compliance with treatment. This statement should be explored and confirmed by further studies—for example, in cerebrovascular risk factor follow up.

    Abbreviations: CL, confidence limit; CNS, central nervous system; PNS, peripheral nervous system

    Keywords: central nervous system diseases; aetiology; patient compliance; adaptation; psychological; doctor–patient relations

    Patients are often unaware of, or do not understand about, recent developments in molecular and genetic medicine and more technical interventional therapies. Thus when patients search for explanations for triggers and the evolution of their disorder they often blame, for instance, psychological factors or special events.

    We hypothesised that this could result in divergence of opinion between the patient and the doctor, which, in turn, might lead to poor compliance with the proposed treatment.1,2 A British study of patients in general practice showed that only half the patients thought they would benefit from medical treatment or hospitalisation for investigations.3 The failure of conventional medicine to take into account psychological aspects of illness may be one reason for the recent enthusiasm for alternative and complementary therapies.4–6 Another reason for evaluating patients’ beliefs about psychological factors is that these factors have been described as a possible influence on the triggering of diseases.7 In addition, there is recent evidence for a beneficial effect of spiritual belief on the evolution of chronic disorders and on patients’ ability to cope with the disease.8,9 Stress and excessive workload have also been recently recognised as possible factors in the development of chronic pain syndrome, with or without anatomical lesions.10,11

    To evaluate these aspects of clinical practice, we asked patients to reply orally to a short questionnaire during the first two days after admission to a neurology inpatient department.

    PATIENTS AND METHODS

    Of 750 consecutive inpatients admitted to our neurology department between November 2001 and June 2002, 41 patients refused to answer the short questionnaire and 367 were unable to answer within two days of admission (critically ill, demented, confused, severely aphasic, and anosognosic patients). The remaining 342 patients (49% women, 51% men; mean (SD) age 61 (19) years) participated in the study. These patients answered a short questionnaire consisting of three questions:

    The first question was open ended to evaluate spontaneous answers: "What do you think triggered your disease?"

    The second question assessed psychological factors in general: "Do you think that psychological factors played a role in triggering it?"

    The third question explored whether the following factors could have triggered the patient’s disease: stress, fatigue, excessive work or other activities, poor lifestyle, conflict with another person, tragic event, chance, and destiny.

    The diagnosis was made on the basis of a standard, generally

    RESULTS

    When answering the first open ended question, 49% of patients included in the study spontaneously admitted that they had no idea of what could have caused their disease (95% confidence limit (CL) 43.7% to 54.3%), 15% gave a congruent medical explanation (95% CL 11.2% to 18.8%), 11% mentioned stress and fatigue as a precipitating factor (95% CL 7.7% to 14.3%), 6% mentioned a psychological event or a dysthymia (95% CL 3.5% to 8.5%), and 6% gave a non-congruent medical explanation (95% CL 3.5% to 8.5%). Some answers could not be classified into any of the aforementioned categories: one patient said that he was convinced that a portable phone antenna placed next to his house was responsible for triggering his transient aphasia symptoms, and another patient, who had had a right middle cerebral artery stroke, blamed the shock of France’s election in 2002 when the far right candidate qualified for a second run.

    When asked if psychological factors were responsible for triggering their disease, 36% of patients gave an affirmative answer (95% CL 30.9% to 41.1%). Internal factors, such as stress (48%, 95% CL 42.7% to 53.3%) and fatigue (51%, 95% CL 45.7% to 56.3%), and those factors which cannot be influenced, such as chance (54%, 95% CL 48.7% to 59.3%) and destiny (43%, 95% CL 37.8% to 48.2%), were blamed more often than external factors, such as excess work or other activities (30%, 95% CL 25.1% to 34.9%), poor lifestyle (16%, 95% CL 12.1% to 19.9%), conflict with another person (14%, 95% CL 10.3% to 17.7%), or a tragic event (18%, 95% CL 13.9% to 22.1%). Significantly different proportions of patients from the different diagnostic groups considered psychological factors as a triggering factor (fig 1A; 2 test, p = 0.026), mainly due to the small proportion of patients with PNS diseases and stroke who blamed these factors. In the group of patients with stroke (fig 1B–D), no significant differences were found between the sexes (81 women, 96 men) (2 test, p = 0.21), among different stroke locations (2 test, p = 0.43), and between the two age groups, despite a strong tendency for younger patients (<65 years, n = 54) to evoke these factors more frequently than older patients (>65 years, n = 123) (2 test, p = 0.06). The age distribution was not significantly different among the subgroups based on stroke location (right internal carotid artery (ICA, n = 48), 67 (18) years; left ICA, n = 54, 71 (13) years; vertebrobasilar, n = 75, 69 (13), ANOVA, p = 0.76) and between the sexes (women, 71 (15) years; men, 67 (15) years, Student’s t test, p = 0.08).

    Figure 1 Percentage of affirmative answers for psychological factors. (A) Comparison between different diagnostic groups. (B–D) Comparison of the patients with stroke by (B) age, (C) sex, and (D) location of stroke (for statistics, see text). Left ICA, left internal carotid artery territory; PNS, peripheral nervous system; right ICA, right internal carotid artery territory; VB, vertebrobasilar territory.

    The proportion of patients in the different diagnostic groups who considered stress (2 test, p = 0.029) as a triggering factor was significantly different, mainly due to the high proportion of patients with chronic primary headache blaming this factor. A significant difference was also found for destiny (2 test, p = 0.043), mainly due to a low frequency of affirmative answers in the PNS diseases group (table 1).

    Table 1 Factors blamed by the different groups (%)

    DISCUSSION

    As the understanding of disease mechanisms and the diagnostic tools used become more complex and specialised, patients’ may only partially comprehend their disease. Some beliefs of patients may overlap those of their doctors and some may not, and this could influence their compliance with therapy.1,2 In our study, only 15% of patients gave a congruent medical explanation when asked about what could have caused their disease and more than a third believed that psychological factors had played a role in triggering it. Only 16% of stroke patients mentioned a poor lifestyle, whereas many studies have described environmental and lifestyle factors as important in causing cerebrovascular diseases.12,13 In contrast, chance and destiny were frequently mentioned by the patients, which, at least for cerebrovascular disease, does not agree with the medical opinion that genetic, environmental, and lifestyle factors are responsible for the development of the majority of cerebrovascular events.

    On the other hand, some studies have described psychological factors in triggering of various diseases such as coronary heart disease,14 fatigue and excessive work as important in the regulation of biological parameters, such as lipid profiles,15 and psychological events as modulators of immune response.16 Other studies have evoked stress and fatigue as worsening factors for headaches and chronic CNS diseases, respectively.17–19

    In summary, patients’ beliefs about factors triggering neurological disease only partially concur with medical opinion. If doctors paid particular attention to their beliefs when explaining the medical point of view regarding triggering of their disease, patients’ compliance with treatment could improve. A future development may be to repeat this study with patients with heart (coronary artery) diseases and compare the results with those of the present study. We would expect the results from such a study to be closer to those of our patients with PNS diseases than those of the patients with CNS diseases. Asking patients’ relatives the same questions could also provide new insights into patient care, as relatives often influence compliance with therapy. It should also be confirmed whether patients’ beliefs influence compliance—for example, in cerebrovascular risk factor follow up after stroke.

    REFERENCES

    Nicholl JS. Noncompliance with treatment of neurologic disease. Curr Treat Options Neurol 2002;4:469–76.

    Wiles R, Kinmonth A. Patients’ understandings of heart attack: implications for prevention of recurrence. Patient Educ Couns 2001;44:161–9.

    Woloshynowych M, Valori R, Salmon P. General practice patients’ beliefs about their symptoms. Br J Gen Pract 1998;48:885–9.

    Rajendran PR, Thompson RE, Reich SG. The use of alternative therapies by patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 2001;57:790–4.

    Schwartz CE, Laitin E, Brotman S, et al. Utilization of unconventional treatments by persons with MS: is it alternative or complementary? Neurology 1999;52:626–9.

    Eisenberg DM, Davis RB, Ettner SL, et al. Trends in alternative medicine use in the United States, 1990–1997: results of a follow-up national survey. JAMA 1998;280:1569–75.

    Olff M . Stress, depression and immunity: the role of defense and coping styles. Psychiatry Res 1999;85:7–15.

    Mueller PS, Plevak DJ, Rummans TA. Religious involvement, spirituality, and medicine: implications for clinical practice. Mayo Clin Proc 2001;76:1225–35.

    Gundersen L . Faith and healing. Ann Intern Med 2000;132:169–72.

    Bongers PM, de Winter CR, Kompier MA, et al. Psychosocial factors at work and musculoskeletal disease. Scand J Work Environ Health 1993;19:297–312.

    Hoogendoorn WE, van Poppel MN, Bongers PM, et al. Systematic review of psychosocial factors at work and private life as risk factors for back pain. Spine 2000;25:2114–25.

    Hankey GJ. Risk factor management to prevent stroke. Adv Neurol 2003;92:179–85.

    Luedemann J, Schminke U, Berger K, et al. Association between behavior-dependent cardiovascular risk factors and asymptomatic carotid atherosclerosis in a general population. Stroke 2002;33:2929–35.

    Hemingway H, Marmot M. Evidence based cardiology: psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease. Systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMJ 1999;318:1460–7.

    Koertge JC, Ahnve S, Schenck-Gustafsson K, et al. Vital exhaustion in relation to lifestyle and lipid profile in healthy women. Int J Behav Med 2003;10:44–55.

    Bower JE, Kemeny ME, Taylor SE, et al. Finding positive meaning and its association with natural killer cell cytotoxicity among participants in a bereavement-related disclosure intervention. Ann Behav Med 2003;25:146–55.

    Vos J, Passchier J. Reduced impact of migraine in everyday life: an observational study in the Dutch Society of Headache Patients. Headache 2003;43:645–50.

    Chakravarty A . Chronic daily headaches: clinical profile in Indian patients. Cephalalgia 2003;23:348–53.

    Garber CE, Friedman JH. Effects of fatigue on physical activity and function in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Neurology 2003;60:1119–24.(A Croquelois1,2, G Assal1)