当前位置: 首页 > 期刊 > 《大脑学杂志》 > 2005年第2期 > 正文
编号:11401700
Mapping the brain in autism. A voxel-based MRI study of volumetric dif
http://www.100md.com 《大脑学杂志》
     1 Departments of Psychiatry

    4 Diagnostic Radiology, University of Hong Kong, SAR China,2 Cambridge Brain Mapping Unit, University of Cambridge, UK,3 School of Early Childhood Education, Hong Kong Institute of Education, Hong Kong, SAR China

    5 Department of Psychological Medicine, Institute of Psychiatry, London, UK

    Summary

    Autism is a disorder of neurodevelopment resulting in pervasive abnormalities in social interaction and communication, repetitive behaviours and restricted interests. There is evidence for functional abnormalities and metabolic dysconnectivity in ‘social brain’ circuitry in this condition, but its structural basis has proved difficult to establish reliably. Explanations for this include replication difficulties inherent in ‘region of interest’ approaches usually adopted, and variable inclusion criteria for subjects across the autism spectrum. Moreover, despite a consensus that autism probably affects widely distributed brain regions, the issue of anatomical connectivity has received little attention. Therefore, we planned a fully automated voxel-based whole brain volumetric analysis in children with autism and normal IQ. We predicted that brain structural changes would be similar to those previously shown in adults with autism spectrum disorder and that a correlation analysis would suggest structural dysconnectivity. We included 17 stringently diagnosed children with autism and 17 age-matched controls. All children had IQ >80. Using Brain Activation and Morphological Mapping (BAMM) software, we measured global brain and tissue class volumes and mapped regional grey and white matter differences across the whole brain. With the expectation that volumes of interconnected regions correlate positively, we carried out a preliminary exploration of ‘connectivity’ in autism by comparing the nature of inter-regional grey matter volume correlations with control. Children with autism had a significant reduction in total grey matter volume and significant increase in CSF volume. They had significant localized grey matter reductions within fronto-striatal and parietal networks similar to findings in our previous study, and additional decreases in ventral and superior temporal grey matter. White matter was reduced in the cerebellum, left internal capsule and fornices. Correlation analysis revealed significantly more numerous and more positive grey matter volumetric correlations in controls compared with children with autism. Thus, using similar diagnostic criteria and image analysis methods in otherwise healthy populations with an autistic spectrum disorder from different countries, cultures and age groups, we report a number of consistent findings. Taken together, our data suggest abnormalities in the anatomy and connectivity of limbic–striatal ‘social’ brain systems which may contribute to the brain metabolic differences and behavioural phenotype in autism.

    Key Words: autism; MRI; brain mapping

    Abbreviations: BA = Brodmann area; ROI = region of interest; STS = superior temporal sulcus; VBM = voxel-based morphometry

    Introduction

    Autism is a highly genetic neurodevelopmental disorder (Folstein and Rutter, 1977; Bailey et al., 1996), characterized by a triad of repetitive and stereotypic behaviour, impaired communication and striking deficits in social reciprocity. Post-mortem studies date the onset of neuropathology to as early as the second half of pregnancy (Kemper and Bauman, 2002), and there is increasing evidence that this results in anatomical abnormalities in fronto-temporo-parietal cortices, the limbic system and cerebellum, but many findings have not been replicated (Bailey et al., 1996; Brambilla et al., 2003). This is probably due in part to differences between studies in diagnostic criteria used and confounders such as the age, IQ and physical health of people studied which impact upon brain structure. These factors hold particular relevance to autism as postnatal brain maturation is altered in autism (Courchesne et al., 2001; Aylward et al., 2002; McAlonan et al., 2002), a majority of people with autism have low IQ and many also have epilepsy. However, failure to replicate may also be a consequence of the methods of data acquisition and analysis and many prior studies have used ‘region of interest (ROI)’ manual tracing approaches which are not easily reproducible across different laboratories (Brambilla et al., 2003).

    It is relatively unlikely that the behavioural phenotype of autism (or any complex neurodevelopmental disorder) can be explained by abnormalities in one brain region. In the general population, studies of social understanding (specifically theory of mind tasks) implicate prefrontal cortex, medial and ventral temporal lobe, superior temporal sulcus (STS), amygdala and cerebellum (Fletcher et al., 1995; Calarge et al., 2003; Schultz et al., 2003) as candidate brain determinants of autism. Theory of mind tasks do not activate all these regions in adults with autism (Happe et al., 1996; Baron-Cohen et al., 1999; Castelli et al., 2001) and, although the early processing in extra-striate visual areas appears normal in individuals with autism, significantly reduced functional connectivity between these areas and STS has been reported (Castelli et al., 2001). Horwitz et al. (1988) used PET to address metabolic connectivity in autism and reported significant differences in cortico-subcortical connectivity. Therefore, it is plausible that the anatomy, connectivity and function of brain systems are affected by autism. In a preliminary assessment of the organization of brain systems in autism, Herbert et al. (2003) combined structural MRI data from boys with autism and their healthy controls in a factor analysis study (grouping volumes which co-vary or inter-correlate together). They derived three main factor groupings: central white matter; cerebral cortex and hippocampus–amygdala; and other grey matter regions; and found that autism appeared to drive white matter to be larger and cerebral cortex and hippocampus–amygdala to be smaller. However, again this was limited by an ROI approach so that more subtle anatomical differences in distributed neural networks could not be examined a priori.

    Thus there is a need for detailed anatomical studies of (relatively) homogeneous populations employing newer, more sensitive techniques such as automated voxel-based morphometry (VBM) of whole brain (Brambilla et al., 2003). In the first study of this type, Abell et al. (1999) used statistical parametric mapping to explore grey matter differences in autism. They found grey matter volume reductions in prefrontal lobe and increases in temporal lobe and cerebellum. We subsequently used a non-parametric VBM approach in a study of autistic spectrum adults of normal intelligence in the UK and reported rather more extensive grey matter reductions across frontal, limbic, basal ganglia, parietal and cerebellar regions as well as significant volume changes in related white matter tracts (McAlonan et al., 2002). We concluded that adults with an autistic spectrum disorder have regional differences not only in brain volume, but probably also in anatomical organization of large-scale brain systems. However, we did not know if our findings generalized to other autistic populations (i.e. to children or other cultures) and we were unable to examine the anatomical ‘connectivity’ of neural systems which putatively underlie the disorder.

    Therefore, we examined the brain anatomy of high functioning Chinese children with classical autism at normal schools. We compared these with control children from the same narrow age range and socio-economic class. We used MRI and non-parametric VBM to identify significant between-group differences in the whole brain and regional volumes of grey and white matter and CSF. Interconnecting brain systems have common developmental and maturation influences (Cheverud, 1982, 1984), thus their volumes would be expected to co-vary or positively correlate (Kerwin and Murray, 1992; Bullmore et al., 1998; Zhang and Sejnowski, 2000). We therefore predicted that, compared with controls, children with autism would have disrupted connectivity reflected by fewer and less positive grey matter volume correlation coefficients within the affected grey matter network.

    Methods

    Subjects

    Subject characteristics are shown in Table 1. All subjects were ethnic Chinese, right-handed, with IQ >80 (estimated using Raven's progressive matrices). We excluded children with a co-morbid psychiatric or medical condition (e.g. epilepsy), history of head injury or genetic disorder associated with autism (e.g. tuberous sclerosis or fragile X syndrome). Seventeen (16 male, one female) were non-medicated children with autism aged 8–14 years, recruited from a community autism programme. ICD10 diagnosis of autism was confirmed using the 1994 Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised (Lord et al., 1994) translated into Chinese in-house. Seventeen (16 male, one female) typically developing control children aged 8–14 years old were recruited from local schools and screened for major psychiatric illness using a structured parental interview. We prioritized age matching of groups, as a lower IQ is a recognized complication of autism (Happe and Frith, 1996). Every child's parent gave informed consent for the protocol approved by the University of Hong Kong Faculty of Medicine Research Ethics Committee, and each child gave his/her assent.

    MRI and analysis

    Using slices of 3 mm thickness, dual-echo fast spin echo data sets aligned to the anterior commissure–posterior commissure (AC–PC) line were acquired across the whole brain on a GE Sigma 1.5 T system (General Electric, Milwaukee, WI) in Queen Mary Hospital, Hong Kong. A consultant radiologist (K.S.T.), blind to diagnosis, reviewed each MRI scan, and none was found to have any gross anomaly.

    Group differences in grey and white matter were mapped using the BAMM software (Brain Analysis Morphological Mapping version 2.5, http://www-bmu.psychiatry.cam.ac.uk/BAMM/index.html) on a SPARC workstation (Sun Microsystems Europe Inc., Surrey, UK). This process has been described previously for both adult and child populations (Overmeyer et al., 2001; Sigmundsson et al., 2001; McAlonan et al., 2002). Briefly, images were processed to remove extra-cerebral tissues (Suckling et al., 1999a) and then segmented into grey and white matter, CSF and a fourth class including dura, vessels and other extraneous tissues which subsequently were ignored (Suckling et al., 1999b). Global volumes of grey and white matter, CSF and whole brain were calculated and compared across groups using independent t tests. The segmented images were mapped into the standard space of Talairach and Tournoux (1988) by minimizing the sum of square intensity difference of each proton density image to a group-specific template (mean of three children with autism and three controls; Brammer et al., 1997) and smoothed with a 4.4 mm kernel. The effect of diagnostic group was estimated at each voxel by regression of a general linear model. Maps of the appropriate normalized coefficient were subject to an inference procedure in which the significance of three-dimensional cluster statistics was assessed using non-parametric methods (Bullmore et al., 1999). The statistical thresholds were corrected for multiple comparisons by controlling the ‘family wise error rate’, in this case by setting the P value used such that <1 false-positive cluster was expected under the null hypothesis. A cluster of grey or white matter abnormality was defined as ‘deficit’ or ‘excess’ depending on whether the volume was reduced or increased in the autism group relative to the control group. To determine whether the IQ difference between groups had an impact on our results, we entered the volumes derived in a multiple analysis of covariance (MANCOVA; SPSS 11.5.1 general linear model, multivariate analysis) with IQ as a covariate.

    Correlation analysis

    Volumetric correlations across subjects were explored between each pair of clusters of significant grey matter difference using Pearson's correlations in both groups separately. The number of intra-regional correlations with r 0.4 found was compared with the 2 square distribution. We also used randomization to assess non-parametrically the significance of between-group differences for each inter-regional correlation. This procedure involved repeated (400) random reassignments of each individual's set of volume measures to diagnostic group to generate correlation matrices which sample the null hypothesis that group differences occurred by chance (Woodruff et al., 1997; Bullmore et al., 1998). To test our prediction that correlation coefficients in the control group would be more positive than the autism group, we used the 99 percentile point as a critical value for a two-tailed test of the null distribution with probability of type 1 error 0.01.

    Results

    Quantitative structural MRI

    The total volume of grey matter in the autism group was significantly reduced [t(32) = 3.13, P < 0.005] and total volume of CSF significantly increased [t(32) = –2.8, P < 0.005]. There were no overall group differences in total brain, nor total white matter volumes (see Table 2).

    Regional grey matter differences

    Subjects with autism had on average 25% reduction in 13, three-dimensional grey matter clusters. In the right hemisphere, deficit clusters were observed in the orbital, inferior and middle frontal gyri, caudate nucleus, ventral temporal lobe and medial parietal lobe. In the left hemisphere, the deficit clusters were located in orbital, middle and medial frontal gyri, middle and superior temporal gyri, caudate nucleus and medial parietal lobe (cluster threshold P = 0.01, cluster test significance P = 0.002; Fig. 1 and Table 3). Co-varying IQ (MANCOVA) did not alter the significance of group volume differences.

    Regional white matter differences

    There were two extensive 3D clusters of white matter reduction in autism. White matter was reduced bilaterally in the cerebellum by 19% and in the left internal capsule and bilateral fornices by 21%. At the level of significance selected, no differences in corpus callosum volumes were observed (cluster threshold P = 0.05, cluster test significance P = 0.002; Table 3 and Fig. 2). Co-varying IQ (MANCOVA) did not alter the significance of volume differences.

    Correlation analyses

    Results are shown in detail in Table 4. Pearson correlation analysis of 3D clusters revealed that, out of a possible 78 correlations in each group, there were large positive correlation coefficients (r 0.4) in 24 pairs in the control group and only one sizeable negative coefficient (r –0.4). In contrast, only eight pairs in the autism group had substantial positive inter-regional volumetric correlations (r 0.4), while two pairs were strongly negatively correlated (r –0.4). The number of positive correlations in the control group was significantly greater than in the autism group [2(1) = 10.06, P < 0.002]. Twenty-three correlation coefficients in the control group, mostly where r 0.4, were significantly larger than those in the autism group (P < 0.01); only five correlation coefficients in the autism group (four, where r 0.4) were significantly larger than the corresponding control values. The multiple large positive correlations in the control group were both inter- and intrahemispheric and often involved the caudate nuclei and medial parietal lobe. Positive correlations in the autism group involved the right ventral temporal lobe, left temporal lobe [Brodmann area (BA) 22], prefrontal lobe (BA8 and right BA9), precuneus and the right caudate nucleus. Bonferroni adjustment for multiple comparisons was not applied (Perneger, 1998), as correlation matrices indicated that observations were not independent.

    Discussion

    Using a voxel-based analysis in a group of intellectually able children with autism, we found they had reduced global grey matter volumes and increased CSF volumes. Whole brain volume was unchanged, and this agrees with previous reports that megaloencephaly is not a feature of autism after age 4 years (Courchesne et al., 2001; Aylward et al., 2002; McAlonan et al., 2002). Our principal findings, however, were that children with autism had significant differences in the regional brain volume of prefrontal and parieto-temporal cortices which are considered to underpin ‘social cognition’ (Brothers, 1990) and language (Binder, 1997; Binder et al., 1997). In addition, we found marked disruption to cortico-subcortical and cortico-cortical grey matter volumetric relationships which mirror the reduction in cortico-subcortical and cortico-cortical metabolic correlations in autism recorded by Horwitz et al. (1988) and add weight to the concept that people with autism spectrum disorders have differences in the anatomical and functional integration of large-scale neural systems.

    Abnormalities in a social brain network

    The distribution of grey matter deficits in the autism group is striking in the extent to which it encompasses brain regions with critical socio-emotional function. For example, the right fusiform gyrus, concerned with face recognition (Schultz et al., 2003), and the STS, with judging changeable aspects of face such as eye gaze, expression and lip movement (Haxby et al., 2002), may be core to face analysis. The orbitofrontal cortex forms an interface between emotion and cognition (Rolls, 2004) and, together with the left middle temporal lobe, precuneus and posterior cingulate, is implicated in making social judgements and empathy (Farrow et al., 2001). Finally, the medial prefrontal lobe (BA8) permits an understanding of the mental state of others, or theory of mind (Fletcher et al., 1995; Frith and Frith, 1999). These results stand in dramatic contrast to a recent VBM study of Williams syndrome which showed that, in this neurodevelopmental disorder of heightened socio-emotional responsiveness, grey matter was increased in a similar circuit through bilateral orbital gyri, left middle temporal gyrus, right fusiform, precuneus and cingulate regions (Reiss et al., 2004). Thus, at the very simplest level of interpretation, there appears to be a dichotomy between developmental pressures which increase grey matter in a social brain network and bias towards socio-emotional behaviour, while grey matter reduction in the same system impairs social interaction.

    The network of grey matter change in autism detailed here therefore provides a plausible basis for the differences in brain activation during social and communicative tasks we and others have reported in autism in the medial prefrontal cortex (Happe et al., 1996), superior temporal gyrus (Critchley et al., 2000; Schultz et al., 2000; Just et al., 2004) and ventral temporal lobe (Schultz et al., 2000; Pierce et al., 2001). For instance, the location of the medial prefrontal cortex (BA8) and posterior cingulate (BA31) deficits described here corresponds closely to the areas of activation observed during a ‘theory of mind’ task in typical adult subjects (Fletcher et al., 1995) but not in subjects with autism performing the same task (Happe et al., 1996). Similarly, the right fusiform gyrus in the ventral temporal lobe is activated reliably during face recognition, except in individuals with autism (Pierce et al., 2004). Where estimations of functional ‘connectivity’ have been made, these suggest network disintegration in autism (Horwitz et al., 1988; Schultz et al., 2000; Castelli et al., 2001; Just et al., 2004) but the structural basis is unclear. Since interconnecting brain regions exert mutually trophic effects during development, their volumes could be expected to correlate positively (Kerwin and Murray, 1992; Bullmore et al., 1998). Therefore, we suggest that the relative absence of positive inter-regional volumetric correlations in autism points to widespread structural dysconnectivity within the social brain in this condition. We did, however, observe a very few large correlation pairs in autism which were significantly more positive than in the control group. For example, although the volume of the right ventral temporal lobe/fusiform gyrus was reduced in our autism group, it remained strongly correlated with the volume of BA8/medial prefrontal lobe. In typically developing individuals, the fusiform gyrus may be activated preferentially by faces simply because there is strong social motivation to acquire ‘expertise’ in faces (Gauthier et al., 1999; Grelotti et al., 2002). In the absence of this social drive in autism, the fusiform face area may yet be engaged by non-face (expert) objects (Grelotti et al., 2002) and so establish links to components of an otherwise pathological network. Similarly, the volumes of abnormal left middle and superior temporal gyri clusters are strongly correlated in the autism group. The BA22 region is close to an area of unusually increased activity noted by Just et al. (2004) in individuals with autism during a sentence completion task. They considered this might reflect hyperlexical skills for single word processing in autism. Thus, against a background of generalized network disintegration in autism, isolated connections of the kind described here might contribute to a neural substrate of aspects of autism which are not ‘deficit’ symptoms.

    Neuropathological basis of autism

    Exploring inter-regional volume correlations may additionally provide a window onto patterns of very early development (Bullmore et al., 1998). Subcortical afferentation of the cortex (from 20 weeks gestation) depends upon controlled neuronal migration from periventricular regions across a striatopallidal ‘boundary zone’ (Molnar and Butler, 2002) and is followed by cortico-cortical afferentation. Thus the absence of inter-regional volumetric correlations, together with the particular distribution of cortico-subcortical grey matter anomalies and white matter abnormalities in childhood autism demonstrated here, could reflect afferentation problems during fetal brain development. Consistent with this, post-mortem studies date structural malformations in autism to early in fetal life (Bailey et al., 1998) possibly prior to 28 weeks (Kemper and Bauman, 2002). Of most immediate clinical interest, this account bolsters the evidence that brain abnormalities in autism are measurable before the possible impact of postulated ‘causal’ postnatal events such as vaccinations (Courchesne et al., 2001). However, we do not intend to suggest that aberrant fetal development can explain all the neuroanatomical changes reported here. Rather we appreciate that brain morphology and organization of its circuitry continues throughout childhood (Munte et al., 2002) by ‘interactive specialization’ (Johnson, 2000, 2003). Brain dysmaturation in autism is therefore likely to be on-going and is reflected by age-related changes in global brain or tissue class volumes (Courchesne et al., 2001; Aylward et al., 2002; McAlonan et al., 2002). Thus our results are therefore most likely to be caused by a combination of factors, including an initial neurobiological ‘insult’ to neural networks which are modified further after birth.

    Previous VBM studies

    This study of high functioning Chinese children in part replicates earlier work in adults with Asperger's syndrome in the UK which adopted the same VBM approach (McAlonan et al., 2002). It appears that a core of bilateral grey matter anomalies in prefrontal, caudate and medial parietal areas generalize across different age and ethnic groups who are physically healthy and not learning disabled. However, while the VBM technique provided detailed information about possible abnormalities in this spectrum in our two studies, the results do not overlap fully. In particular, compared with our previous work, here we found grey matter reductions in right ventral temporal lobe and left STS, but no extensive change to putamen or cerebellar grey matter volumes and fewer white matter changes (McAlonan et al., 2002). This is not entirely surprising given a number of important differences between the two studies. The present study addressed classical autism, not Asperger's. We recruited children in this study, whereas the previous study involved adults. Childhood and adolescence are very dynamic periods of brain modulation; white matter myelination increases linearly by 12% between the ages of 4 and 22 years (Giedd et al., 1999) while grey matter decreases by 4–9% during this period (Giedd et al., 1999; Sowell et al., 2002). Given the cross-sectional design of both our studies, the age of participants probably influenced the precise pattern of results. Moreover, participants in our current study were native Chinese speakers and bilingual for Chinese and English, while in the previous study they were native English speakers. Subtle structural anatomical differences in the frontal, temporal and parietal lobes have been reported between Chinese and Caucasians (Kochunov et al., 2003) and learning two languages appears to recruit distinct language modules (Chee et al., 2000). Chinese language experience impacts upon the haemodynamic response in the right ventral temporal lobe (Tan et al., 2000), left superior temporal gyrus (BA22; Tan et al., 2001) and left middle frontal cortex (BA9/46; Tan et al., 2003) and we found group difference in grey matter volume in each of these regions in the current study but not in our previous study. Children with autism have significant language difficulties, so we postulate that the particular language demands of living in Hong Kong modulate the brain and compound the fronto-temporal abnormalities noted in children with autism in the present study.

    The present results were much more at odds with a VBM study which described increased grey matter volumes in the temporal lobe and the cerebellum (Abell et al., 1999). Since the patients in Abell's study were similar to our UK study, methodological differences may be relatively more important. In the study of Abell et al., segmentation of single spectrum images was assisted by determining the probability that voxel values were grey matter (white matter was not addressed). Images were then normalized to the standard MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) template and smoothed with a 12 mm kernel before parametric testing. Arguably, pre-processing and analysis of our data relied upon fewer assumptions, as it involved bi-modality segmentation, smoothing with a smaller kernel and non-parametric permutation test statistics (Bullmore et al., 1999). In particular, by normalizing images to a group-specific template which incorporated patients, we hoped to introduce less bias (Good et al., 2002). However, type 1 error control (number of false positives <1) in our study was particularly stringent and, while this lends confidence to the results, it does increase the chance of false-negative results (type II error) which may explain some of the discrepancies between studies.

    Methodological considerations

    Other groups have used ROI measurements of structural MRI and sophisticated factor analytical methods to describe the organization of brain systems in neurodevelopmental disorders such as schizophrenia (Wright et al., 1999) and autism (Herbert et al., 2003). This allows grouping of brain regions also according to their volumetric intercorrelations. However, statistical considerations meant that in both examples the authors combined patient and control data sets. We believe that these approaches are not applicable to the present data set since the absolute regional volumes we derived by the VBM analysis are very small. Effectively, as subcomponents of the larger regions measureable using ROI techniques, their impact would probably be diluted. In addition, the decision to combine two groups of modest size to improve statistical power rests upon the assumption that brain regions are organized in the same way. We predicted that this would not be the case in autism, and the marked differences between group correlation matrices agree that this assumption would not have been valid for our data set.

    There are a number of limitations to our work. First, we acknowledge that our study only addressed a subset of the autism spectrum and the number of subjects was modest, therefore the results may not apply to the entire population with this condition. The correlation approach we used depends upon the assumption that positive volumetric correlations indicate connectivity. While this seems a logical interpretation, we cannot say for certain that it is valid. Also, although we found only three sizeable negative correlations in our analysis (two in the autism group and one in the control group), it is difficult to know what they might reflect. Possibly brain maturation leads to a measure of reorganization of neural systems with some connections strengthened at the expense of others. With the advent of diffusion tensor imaging techniques, more direct examination of connectivity is now possible and should help clarify these issues. Finally, a major problem with correlation analysis is that an apparent correlation may exist because both data points in a pair correlate with a third rather than each other. Nevertheless, we present the correlation analysis as a fresh perspective on structural MRI data which may extend our understanding about the development of brain networks in autism and other disorders of neurodevelopment.

    Conclusions

    Our main finding is that the anatomy of brain systems implicated in social, emotional and communicative behaviour is disrupted in autism. Within this system, core prefrontal–striato-parietal grey matter abnormalities in autism may be replicable in age-matched and intellectually able groups, using automated voxel-based whole brain analysis methods. We considered correlation analysis as a proxy of network organization and provide preliminary neuroimaging evidence for structural dysconnectivity in the ‘social brain’ in autism.

    Acknowledgements

    We wish to thank Professors Edward Bullmore and Pak Sham for their valuable advice on study design and analysis, and two anonymous reviewers for their comprehensive comments which greatly improved the manuscript. This work was supported by a University of Hong Kong Seed fund to S.E.C.

    References

    Abell F, Krams M, Ashburner J, Passingham R, Friston K, Frackowiak R, et al. The neuroanatomy of autism: a voxel-based whole brain analysis of structural scans. Neuroreport 1999; 10: 1647–51.

    Aylward EH, Minshew NJ, Field K, Sparks BF, Singh N. Effects of age on brain volume and head circumference in autism. Neurology 2002; 59: 175–83.

    Bailey A, Phillips W, Rutter M. Autism: towards an integration of clinical, genetic, neuropsychological, and neurobiological perspectives. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 1996; 37: 89–126.

    Bailey A, Luthert P, Dean A, Harding B, Janota I, Montgomery M, Rutter M, Lantos P. A clinicopathological study of autism. Brain 1998; 121: 889–905.

    Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Wheelwright S, Bullmore ET, Brammer MJ, Simmons A, et al. Social intelligence in the normal and autistic brain: an fMRI study. Eur J Neurosci 1999; 11: 1891–8.

    Binder JR. Neuroanatomy of language processing studied with functional MRI. Clin Neurosci 1997; 4: 87–94.

    Binder JR, Frost JA, Hammeke TA, Cox RW, Rao SM, Prieto T. Human brain language areas identified by functional magnetic resonance imaging. J Neurosci 1997; 17: 353–62.

    Brambilla P, Hardan A, Ucelli di Nemi S, Perez J, Soares JC, Barale F. Brain anatomy and development in autism: review of structural MRI studies. Brain Res Bull 2003; 61: 557–69.

    Brammer M, Bullmore ET, Simmons A, Williams SCR, Grasby PM, Howard RJ, et al. Generic brain activation mapping in fMRI: a nonparametric approach. Magn Reson Imag 1997; 15: 763–70.

    Brothers L. The social brain: a project for integrating primate behaviour and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts Neurosci 1990; 1: 27–151.

    Bullmore ET, Woodruff PWR, Wright IC, Rabe-Hesketh S, Howard RJ, Shuriquie N, et al. Does dysplasia cause anatomical dysconnectivity in schizophrenia Schizophr Res 1998; 30: 127–35.

    Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Rabe-Hesketh S, Taylor E, Brammer MJ. Global, voxel and cluster tests, by theory and permutation for a difference between two groups of structural MR images of the brain. IEEE Trans Med Imag 1999; 18: 32–42.

    Calarge MD, Andreasen NC, O'Leary DS. Visualizing how one brain understands another: a PET study of theory of mind. Am J Psychiatry 2003; 160: 1954–64.

    Castelli F, Frith U, Happe F, Frith CD. Autism and the perception of intentionality in moving geometrical shapes. Neuroimage 2001; 13: S1035.

    Chee MWL, Weekes B, Lee KM, Soon CS, Schreiber A, Hoon JJ, Chee M. Overlap and dissociation of semantic processing of Chinese characters, English words, and pictures: evidence from fMRI. Neuroimage 2000; 12: 392–403.

    Cheverud JM. Phenotypic, genetic, and environmental morphological integration in the cranium. Evolution 1982; 36: 499–516.

    Cheverud JM. Quantative genetics and developmental constraints on evolution by selection. J Theor Biol 1984; 110: 155–71.

    Courchesne E, Karns CM, Davis HR, Chisum HJ, Moses P, Pierce K, et al. Unusual brain growth patterns in early life in patients with autistic disorder—an MRI study. Neurology 2001; 57: 245–54.

    Critchley HD, Daly EM, Bullmore E, Williams SCR, vanAmelsvoort T, Robertson DM, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of social behaviour: changes in cerebral blood flow when high-functioning individuals with autistic disorder process facial expressions. Brain 2000; 123: 2203–12.

    Farrow TFD, Zheng Y, Wilkinson ID, Spence SA, Deakin JFW, Tarrier N, et al. Investigating the functional anatomy of empathy and forgiveness. Neuroreport 2001; 12:11: 2433–8.

    Fletcher PC, Happe F, Frith U, Baker SC, Dolan RJ, Frackowiak RSJ, Frith CD. Other minds in the brain—a functional imaging study of theory of mind in story comprehension. Cognition 1995; 57: 109–28.

    Folstein S, Rutter M. Genetic influences and infantile-autism. Nature 1977; 265: 726–8.

    Frith CD, Frith U. Cognitive psychology—interacting minds—a biological basis. Science 1999; 286: 1692–5.

    Gauthier I, Tarr MJ, Anderson AW, Skudlarski P, Gore JC. Activation of the middle fusiform ‘face area’ increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2: 568–73.

    Giedd JN, Blumenthal J, Jeffries NO, Castellanos FX, Liu H, Zijdenbos A, et al. Brain development during childhood and adolescence: a longitudinal MRI study. Nat Neurosci 1999; 2: 861–3.

    Good CD, Scahill RI, Fox NC, Ashburner J, Friston KJ, Chan D, et al. Automatic differentiation of anatomical patterns in the human brain: validation with studies of degenerative dementias. NeuroImage 2002; 17: 9–36.

    Grelotti DJ, Gauthier 1, Schultz RT. Social interest and the development of cortical face specialization: what autism teaches us about face processing. Dev Psychobiol 2002; 40: 213–25.

    Happe F, Frith U. The neuropsychology of autism. Brain 1996; 119: 1377–400.

    Happe F, Ehlers S, Fletcher P, Frith U, Johansson M, Gillberg C, et al. ‘Theory of mind’ in the brain. Evidence from a PET scan study of Asperger syndrome. Neuroreport 1996; 8: 197–201.

    Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI. Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51: 59–67.

    Herbert MR, Ziegler DA, Deutsch CK, O'Brien LM, Lange N, Bakardjiev A, et al. Dissociations of cerebral cortex, subcortical and cerebral white matter volumes in autistic boys. Brain 2003; 126: 1182–92.

    Horwitz B, Rumsey JM, Grady CL, Rapoport SI. The cerebral metabolic landscape in autism—intercorrelations of regional glucose-utilization. Arch Neurol 1988; 45: 749–55.

    Johnson MH. Functional brain development in infants: elements of an interactive specialization framework. Child Dev 2000; 71: 75–81.

    Johnson MH. Development of human brain functions. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 54: 1312–6.

    Just MA, Cherkassky VL, Keller TA, Minshew NJ. Cortical activation and synchronization during sentence comprehension in high-functioning autism: evidence of underconnectivity. Brain 2004; 127: 1811–21.

    Kemper TL, Bauman ML. Neuropathology of infantile autism. Mol Psychiatry 2002; 7: S12–3.

    Kerwin RT, Murray RM. A developmental perspective on the pathology and neurochemistry of the temporal lobe in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 1992; 7: 1–12.

    Kochunov P, Fox P, Lancaster J, Tan LH, Amunts K, Zilles K, et al. Localized morphological brain differences between English-speaking Caucasians and Chinese-speaking Asians: new evidence of anatomical plasticity. Neuroreport 2003; 14: 961–4.

    Lord C, Rutter M, Lecouteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised—a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord 1994; 24: 659–85.

    McAlonan GM, Daly E, Kumari V, Critchley HD, van Amelsvoort T, Suckling J, et al. Brain anatomy and sensorimotor gating in Asperger's syndrome. Brain 2002; 127: 1594–606.

    Molnar Z, Butler AB. The corticostriatal junction: a crucial region for forebrain development and evolution. Bioessays 2002; 24: 530–41.

    Munte TF, Altenmuller E, Jancke L. The musician's brain as a model of neuroplasticity. Nat Rev Neurosci 2002; 3: 473–8.

    Overmeyer S, Bullmore ET, Suckling J, Simmons A, Williams SCR, Santosh PJ, et al. Distributed grey and white matter deficits in hyperkinetic disorder: MRI evidence for anatomical abnormality in an attentional network. Psychol Med 2001; 31: 1425–35.

    Perneger T. What's wrong with Bonferroni adjustments Br Med J 1998; 316: 1236–8.

    Pierce K, Muller RA, Ambrose J, Allen G, Courchesne E. Face processing occurs outside the fusiform ‘face area’ in autism: evidence from functional MRI. Brain 2001; 124: 2059–73.

    Price JL. Prefrontal cortical networks related to visceral function and mood. Ann NY Acad Sci 1999; 877: 383–96.

    Reiss AL, Eckert MA, Rose FE, Karchemskiy A, Kesler S, Chang M, et al. An experiment of nature: brain anatomy parallels cognition and behaviour in Williams syndrome. J Neurosci 2004; 24: 5009–15.

    Rolls ET. The functions of the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain Cogn 2004; 55: 11–29.

    Schultz RT, Gauthier I, Klin A, Fulbright RK, Anderson AW, Volkmar F, et al. Abnormal ventral temporal cortical activity during face discrimination among individuals with autism and Asperger syndrome. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2000; 57: 331–40.

    Schultz RT, Grelotti DJ, Klin A, Kleinman J, van der Gaag C, Marois R, et al. The role of the fusiform face area in social cognition: implications for the pathobiology of autism. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 2003; 358: 415–27.

    Sigmundsson T, Suckling J, Maier M, Williams SCR, Bullmore ET, Greenwood KE, et al. Structural abnormalities in frontal, temporal, and limbic regions and interconnecting white matter tracts in schizophrenic patients with prominent negative symptoms. Am J Psychiatry 2001; 158: 234–43.

    Sowell ER, Trauner DA, Gamst A, Jernigan TL. Development of cortical and subcortical brain structures in childhood and adolescence: a structural MRI study. Dev Med Child Neurol 2002; 44: 4–16.

    Suckling J, Brammer MJ, Lingford-Hughes A, Bullmore ET. Removal of extracerebral tissues in dual-echo magnetic resonance images via linear scale-space features. Magn Reson Imag 1999a; 17: 247–56.

    Suckling J, Sigmundsson T, Greenwood K, Bullmore ET. A modified fuzzy clustering algorithm for operator independent brain tissue classification of dual echo MR images. Magn Reson Imag 1999b; 17: 1065–76.

    Talairach J, Tournoux P. Co-planar stereotaxic atlas of the human brain. Stuttgart: Thieme; 1988.

    Tan LH, Spinks JA, Gao JH, Liu HL, Perfetti CA, Xiong J, et al. Brain activation in the processing of Chinese characters and words: a functional MRI study. Hum Brain Mapp 2000; 10: 16–27.

    Tan LH, Feng CM, Fox PT, Gao JH, An fMRI study with written Chinese. Neuroreport 2001; 12: 83–8.

    Tan LH, Spinks JA, Feng CM, Siok WT, Perfetti CA, Xiong J, et al. Neural systems of second language reading are shaped by native language. Hum Brain Mapp 2003 18: 158–66.

    Woodruff PWR, Wright IC, Shuriquie N, Russouw H, Rushe T, Howard RJ, et al. Structural brain abnormalities in male schizophrenics reflect fronto-temporal dissociation. Psychol Med 1997; 27: 1257–66.

    Wright IC, Sharma T, Ellison ZR, McGuire PK, Friston KJ, Brammer MJ, et al. Supra-regional brain systems and the neuropathology of schizophrenia. Cereb Cortex 1999; 9: 366–78.

    Zhang K, Sejnowski TJ. A universal scaling law between gray matter and white matter of cerebral cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000; 97: 5621–6.(Grainne M. McAlonan, Vinci Cheung, Charl)